NIH Public Access Author Manuscript J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Published in final edited form as: J Trauma Treat. ; 2: . doi:10.4172/2167-1222.1000167.

Skin conductance response during laboratory stress in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder Helané Wahbeh, ND, MCR1 and Barry Oken, MD1 1Department

of Neurology, Oregon Health & Science University, Portland, Oregon

Abstract Objective—The primary objective was to assess skin conductance response (SCR) to a laboratory stressor in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD) compared to controls. The secondary objective was to evaluate the relationship between SCR and PTSD symptom clusters.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Method—15 combat/ PTSD, 15 combat/no PTSD, 15 no combat/no PTSD veterans had their SCR recording during aversive pictures from the International Affective Picture Scale. Results—The groups had similar demographics and medical history (all p>.05). SCR was different between groups (F(2,42) = 4.34, p=.02). The combat/PTSD group had the highest response compared to both control groups. Numbing-avoiding was predictive of SCR (F(1,42)=12.72, p=.001), while re-experiencing and hyper-arousal were not (p's>.05). Conclusions—Increased SCR in the PTSD group validates previous studies. Numbing-avoiding PTSD cluster scores correlated with SCR values. These findings support current PTSD therapies that reduce avoidance behaviors.

Introduction

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Skin conductance response is a commonly used psychophysiological measure due to the ease of collection and low cost. Skin conductance response (SCR) reflects sympathetic activation because palmar sweat glands are innervated by the sympathetic chain of the autonomic nervous system [1]. There is also a high correlation between bursts of sympathetic nerve activity and SCR [2]. Additionally, activation of cortical emotion regulation areas known to be altered in posttraumatic stress disorder is associated with elicitation of SCR [1]. A number of studies have assessed SCR in people with posttraumatic stress disorder (PTSD). Most studies have observed increased reactivity to a variety of stimuli [3-6] while others have not [6, 7]. However, no study has examined the relationship between SCR and the PTSD symptom clusters of re-experiencing, numbing-avoiding, and hyper-arousal. Assessing the relationship between the individual PTSD symptom components and SCR

Corresponding Author: Helané Wahbeh, ND, MCR, Oregon Health & Science University, 3181 SW Sam Jackson Park Rd., Portland, Oregon 97239, 503-494-3528, [email protected].

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 2

may elucidate which aspects of PTSD psychopathology are most indicative of heightened sympathetic activity.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

The primary objective of this study was to assess SCR in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder compared to combat-exposed and non-combat exposed controls in response to a laboratory stressor. The primary hypothesis was that combat veterans with PTSD would have increased SCR compared to controls. The secondary objective was to evaluate the relationship between SCR and posttraumatic stress disorder symptom clusters. The secondary hypothesis was that increased hyper-arousal symptoms would correlate to increased SCR and re-experiencing and numbing-avoiding would not. Hyper-arousal symptoms represent hyper-activation of the autonomic nervous system and would be expected to increase SCR.

Methods

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Potential participants were recruited through flyers at the Portland Veterans Administration Medical Center, Portland Veterans Center, and other veterans groups throughout the Portland Metropolitan area. The three age and gender-matched participant groups were 15 combat veterans with PTSD, 15 combat veterans without PTSD, and 15 non-combat veterans without PTSD. Veterans were excluded if they were over the age of 70, had a current significant chronic medical illness, bipolar, schizoaffective, or psychotic disorders; any DSM-IV cognitive disorder; substance dependence disorder within 3 months; current substance use other than alcohol (≤2 drinks/day); or sexual assault as primary PTSD event/s. Participants could be on medications (stable doses ≥4 weeks). The study was approved by the Portland Veterans Administration Medical Center and Oregon Health & Science University institutional review boards. Written informed consent was obtained from all participants.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Volunteers were interviewed by trained clinicians using the Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale for DSM-IV (CAPS) [8] to validate the presence or absence of PTSD. Participants in the PTSD group met DSM-IV-TR criteria A-F; criteria B, C, and D were met when the frequency plus intensity score were ≥4. Other traumas were assessed with the Life Events Checklist (LEC), which is administered as part of the CAPS and is used to identify traumatic stressors experienced. It asks about lifetime trauma exposure based on the following scale (1 =happened to me, 2=witnessed it,3=learned about it,4=not sure, and 5 =does not apply) on 16 commonly experienced traumas (Natural disaster, Fire/explosion, Motor vehicle accident, Other serious accident, Exposure to toxic substance, Physical assault, Assault with weapon, Sexual assault, Other unwanted sexual experience, Combat, Captivity, Life-threatening injury/illness, Severe human suffering, Witness violent death, Sudden, unexpected death of loved one, Caused serious injury/death of another, Other very stressful event.) Since the LEC is administered as part of the CAPS to determine trauma exposure for PTSD diagnosis, there is no official way to score the instrument. In order to assess differences between groups on past trauma with this instrument, the number of traumas endorsed where the response was “Happened to me” or “Witnessed It” were totaled for each person (total possible score = 16). Participants in the non-PTSD groups did not meet full PTSD criteria. The Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-Patient Edition (SCID) was performed by the

J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 3

same clinicians to screen for excluded DSM-IV disorders [9]. Combat exposure was determined with the Combat Exposure Scale (CES) [10].

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Study Procedures Study procedures, inclusion/exclusion criteria, and the risks and benefits of participating were explained during a telephone screening. Informed consent procedures, CAPS, SCID, and CES administration were completed at a screening visit. SCR data was collected during a stressor at a laboratory visit scheduled within one week of the screening. The laboratory visit occurred in a light and sound attenuated room, between 10:30am-1:30pm. Participants were seated with a 52 cm monitor placed approximately 60cm away. A constant current (1μA) was applied with two 10mm flat silver/silver chloride electrodes, one on the palmar thenar eminence and the other on the dorsal aspect between the first and second digits. The SCR data was collected with two active Biosemi electrodes (Active 2, Biosemi, Amsterdam, Netherlands) that were next to but not touching the silver/ silver chloride electrodes. All electrodes were placed on the left hand.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Participants were seated for 30 minutes prior to the stressor. The stressor entailed viewing 162 aversive pictures from the International Affective Picture Scale (IAPS) and lasted 13 minutes. Pictures were chosen for their low pleasure, high arousal, and high dominance ratings. The average ratings of all 162 pictures were low pleasure (valence mean=2.1), high arousal (arousal mean=5.8), high dominance (dominance mean=3.8))[11]. The IAPS includes emotionally-evocative color photographs and is known to affect skin conductance [12]. The 162 pictures included four pictures categories: Snake, War, War Control, and Other. The four picture categories were matched for pleasure, arousal, and dominance ratings. War Control pictures were chosen to match the valence, arousal, and dominance ratings of the War pictures (valence 3.0, arousal 5.7, dominance 3.8). There were 6 blocks with 27 pictures per block, where each block had at least 3 Snake, 6 War, 6 War Control, 12 Other and no more than 3 War or 3 War Control in a row. The task was created and presented in EPrime 2.0 (Psychology Software Tools, Inc., Pittsburgh, PA). Pictures were shown for four seconds with a one second inter-stimulus interval. Participant attention was ensured by asking them to click the right hand button upon seeing a snake. After completing the task, participants rated subjective stress on a scale of 1-10, with 10 being the most stressed. The skin conductance signal was digitized at 1024 samples per second and analyzed with Brain Vision Analyzer 2.0 (Brain Products GmbH, Inc., Gilching, Germany). Data was segmented according to stimulus onset (-100ms to 4500ms). Each segment was baseline corrected based on -100ms to stimulus onset. Peak voltages were exported for each segment. Any segment with a peak change greater than 650μV was counted as a response. The number of responses per person was summed (total for all 162 pictures and by picture category) as the primary outcome measure. All statistical analyses were performed with SPSS 18.0 (SPSS, Chicago, IL, USA). Means, standard deviations, and box plots were observed for each continuous variable and normality was assessed by the Shapiro-Wilk test. Group differences in continuous variables were J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 4

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

assessed with analysis of variance (ANOVA) if normally distributed and independent samples Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric test if not normally distributed. Post-hoc tests for significant values were assessed with least square difference for parametric tests and mean rank paired comparisons for non-parametric tests [13]. Categorical variables were assessed with the chi-square test. The primary outcome measure was the SCR for all 162 IAPS pictures (SCR Total). Post-hoc analysis also entailed a repeated measures ANOVA including picture category type (Snake, War, War Control, Other), (group, type, type*group). This was done to see if there was any differene in how the participants responded to picture type both within participant and across groups. Stepwise linear regression analysis was conducted to assess re-experiencing, numbing-avoiding, and hyperarousal symptom components ability to predict SCR. Normality, linearity, and outliers were assessed through normal probability plots and scatter plots of the standardized residuals. None of these assumptions were violated. Zero-order Spearman rank correlation coefficients were examined to determine any collinearity between measures. Criteria probability of F for inclusion in the model was ≤.050, and ≥.100 for exclusion. SCR Total was entered as the dependent measure and re-experiencing, numbing-avoiding, and hyperarousal were entered as independent measures. Confirmatory bivariate correlations were conducted and adjusted with a Bonferroni correction. When comparing pictures categories, percent responded divided by total number of pictures per category was used to account for the different number of pictures in each group (ie War percent equaled number of responses/ 36).

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

Results Demographics and medical history were similar for all groups (all ps>.05 for gender, years in service, era, race, education, marital status, exercise, sleep, handedness, past psychiatric history, past major depressive disorder, antidepressant use, past or current drug, alcohol, or cigarette use, and age (PTSD-55.9 ± 10.4; Combat/no PTSD-51.0 ± 13.0; no combat/ no PTSD- 54.8 ± 9.1). As expected, there were group differences in CAPS, CES, and LEC reflecting PTSD symptomology (Table 1). There was no difference between the two combat groups on the LEC (p=0.26). The group differences were driven by differences between the two combat groups and non-combat group.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

SCR Total was different between groups (F(2,42) = 4.34, p=.02) with the combat, PTSD group having the highest responses to the IAPS aversive pictures overall the whole condition compared to the combat, no PTSD group (p=.007), and the no combat, no PTSD group (p=. 04) (Figure 1). Post-hoc analysis with repeated measures showed no difference in SCR between groups across the picture categories (group interaction; p>.05). There were picture category differences within subjects (F(2,40)=8.98, p=.005). Within each participant's responses, the snake pictures (targets) had significantly greater SCR responses compared to the other categories. There was no difference in SCR response within each participant to the War, War control and Other picture categories. The combat with PTSD group rated the laboratory stressor as more stressful than the control groups (F(2,44)=4.30, p=.02).

J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 5

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

For the linear regression, the numbing-avoiding symptom cluster stayed in the model (F(1,42)=12.72, p=.001), while the re-experiencing and hyper-arousal were excluded (p's>. 05). The collinearity assumption was violated, so bivariate correlations were conducted for all symptom clusters and SCR. Numbing-avoiding (NA) was highly correlated, followed by hyperarousal (HA), and re-experiencing (RE) (NA ρ=.42, p=.005, HA ρ=.33, p=.03, RE ρ=. 29, p=.06). Bonferroni correction renders only numbing-avoiding as significant (.05/3=. 017).

Discussion The primary objective to assess SCR in combat veterans with PTSD compared to controls revealed increased SCR in the PTSD group compared to both trauma and non-trauma exposed controls. This was also reflected in their increased subjective stress. Our results validate other findings that show increased SCR in people with PTSD. There are many methods to assess SCR response. We choose a method that required a specific threshold to be reached before a response was designated for each stimuli. Using this method, rather than magnitude, amplitude, or averaging allowed for a more accurate representation of the response to each stimuli.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript

There were no group differences on any of the picture categories (Snake, War, War Control, and Other). Thus, the combat, PTSD group had a larger SCR to the stressful condition as a whole compared to the other two groups; there was no distinction between being more or less stressed to the different category pictures. Within each participant, the SCR was greater for the Snake pictures compared to the other three picture types. Thus, the participants had a greater stress response to the Snake pictures than the other stressful pictures. The increased response within participants to the Snake pictures probably resulted from the Snake category being a button push target despite SCR being collected on the opposite hand. While the Snake pictures received an increased response within a participant compared to the other picture categories that does not mean that there was no response to the other picture categories. It is just that the Snake category hand a greater response above those. All of the pictures were aversive and “stressful” whether they were War or non-war stimuli. All picture categories were aversive and equal in arousal, valence, and dominance values. This equality in high arousal, negative valence, and high dominance may have overridden any difference in response to the war versus non war content. The increased SCR in the combat, PTSD group reflects increased reactivity to stressful stimuli in general and is not specific to War triggers. This reactivity to stressful triggers that are not related to the person's primary trauma exposure is reflected in the PTSD symptom picture that includes generalized hyperarousal and numbing and avoiding to any stressful trigger (driving, loud noises, etc). This information may be useful for therapies that include exposure focused on the primary PTSD trauma. Additional support to teach autonomic regulation and behavioral modification for any stressful trigger rather than just their primary trigger may also be helpful. The secondary objective was to evaluate the relationship between SCR and PTSD symptom clusters. We hypothesized that hyper-arousal symptoms would positively correlate with

J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 6

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

SCR, while re-experiencing would not. Interestingly, the numbing-avoiding component was the only predictive one in our small study. Numbing-avoiding encompasses the behavioral symptoms of avoiding thoughts, feelings, conversations, people, places, and events that remind the person of their trigger and also the numbing of emotions such as feelings of detachment or estrangement from others or restricted range of affect [14]. People with PTSD who are more avoidant or numb to their emotions may be more reactive to stress and/or triggers, thus perpetuating the avoidant behavior. Hyper-arousal was neither predictive in the regression model nor significantly correlated after correction. The hyper-arousal symptoms reflect hyperreactive sympathetic activity such as difficulty falling or staying asleep, irritability or outbursts of anger, and exaggerated startle response. While people with increased hyper-arousal symptoms may have increased SCR, the numbing-avoiding component had a significant and greater correlation in our small cross-sectional study. Further research with larger sample sizes would help elucidate these relationships more fully.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

The study did have limitations such as small sample size, which made it statistically unfeasible to include covariates in the analysis. It was limited to veterans in a particular age range. Also, the SCR was not collected during a non-stressful task to assess if these differences existed during non-stress. The findings of this study does lend support to current therapies for PTSD that incorporate a reduction of avoidance behaviors, acceptance, or exposure to triggers (whether of their primary PTSD event or other stressful triggers) such as prolonged exposure therapy, dialectical therapy, behavioral activation, and mindfulness meditation. The next step in research is to assess whether this increased SCR to stressors in people with PTSD will change after treatment.

Acknowledgments This work was supported in part by National Institute of Health grants T32 AT002688, K01 AT004951, U19 AT002656, UL1 RR024140, K24AT005121, and a Tartar Trust Grant. Special thanks to Mary Lu, Roger Ellingson, Dan Zajdel, Alex Amen, and Irina Fonareva.

References NIH-PA Author Manuscript

1. Dawson, ME.; Schell, AM.; Filion, DL. The Electrodermal System. In: Cacioppo, JT.; Tassinary, LG.; Berntson, GG., editors. Handbook of Psychophysiology. Cambridge University Press; New York: 2007. p. 159-181. 2. Wallin BG. Sympathetic nerve activity underlying electrodermal and cardiovascular reactions in man. Psychophysiology. 1981; 18(4):470–6. [PubMed: 7267931] 3. McTeague LM, et al. Aversive imagery in posttraumatic stress disorder: trauma recurrence, comorbidity, and physiological reactivity. Biological Psychiatry. 2010; 67(4):346–56. [PubMed: 19875104] 4. Jovanovic T, et al. Altered resting psychophysiology and startle response in Croatian combat veterans with PTSD. International Journal of Psychophysiology. 2009; 71(3):264–8. [PubMed: 19013485] 5. Suendermann O, et al. Early heart rate responses to standardized trauma-related pictures predict posttraumatic stress disorder: a prospective study. Psychosom Med. 2010; 72(3):301–8. [PubMed: 20124426]

J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 7

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

6. Pole N. The psychophysiology of posttraumatic stress disorder: a meta-analysis. Psychol Bull. 2007; 133(5):725–46. [PubMed: 17723027] 7. Blechert J, et al. Autonomic and respiratory characteristics of posttraumatic stress disorder and panic disorder. Psychosom Med. 2007; 69(9):935–43. [PubMed: 17991823] 8. Blake DD, et al. The development of a Clinician-Administered PTSD Scale. J Trauma Stress. 1995; 8(1):75–90. [PubMed: 7712061] 9. First, M., et al. Structured Clinical Interview for DSM-IV-TR Axis 1 Disorders-Patient Edition (SCID-I/P, 11/2002 revision). New York: New York: Biometrics Research Department; 2002. 10. Keane T, et al. Clinical Evaluation of a Measure to Assess Combat Exposure. A Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology. 1989; 1(1):53–55. 11. Lang, P.; Bradley, M.; Cuthbert, B. Technical Report A-6. Gainsville, Fl: University of Florida; 2005. International affective picture system (IAPS): Affective ratings of pictures and instruction manual. 12. Lang PJ, et al. Looking at pictures: affective, facial, visceral, and behavioral reactions. Psychophysiology. 1993; 30(3):261–73. [PubMed: 8497555] 13. Langley, R. Practical Statistics Simply Explained. London: Pan Books; 1979. 14. American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and Statistical Manual of Mental Disorders. 4th. Washington DC: American Psychiatric Publishing Inc; 1994.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 8

NIH-PA Author Manuscript NIH-PA Author Manuscript Figure 1. Skin conductance response between groups

Mean number of SCRs +/- SE out of 162 total stimuli. The combat/PTSD group had higher responses than the combat/no PTSD group (p=.007) and the no combat/no PTSD group (p=. 04). There was no difference between the two control groups p>.05).

NIH-PA Author Manuscript J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Wahbeh and Oken

Page 9

Table 1

Measures by group

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

For CAPS, Combat Exposure Scale, SCR total and stress, the values are mean ± standard deviation. SCR Total has two less participants due to bad signal data (Combat PTSD n=14, non-combat, non-PTSD n=14). CAPS-Clinician administered post-traumatic stress disorder scale; SCR-Skin Conductance Response. Measure

Combat/ PTSD (n=15)

Combat/ Non-PTSD (n=15)

Non-Combat/ Non-PTSD (n=15)

Statistics

75.3 ± 13.2

11.6 ± 11.8

10.9 ± 14.1

X2=26.5; p=.0001a

Re-experiencing

20.2 ± 7.0

1.7 ± 2.7

4.2 ± 6.0

X2=26.7; p=.0001 a

Numbing-Avoiding

31.5 ± 7.5

5.0 ±6.1

4.4 ± 7.9

X2=28.2; p=.0001 a

Hyper-arousal

23.9 ± 4.0

6.4 ± 5.8

2.3 ± 4.5

X2=0.88; p=.0001 a

Combat Exposure Scale

29.6 ± 10.1

18.9 ± 8.8

0.5 ± 1.1

X2=0.88; p=.0001 a

Life Event Checklist

13.5 ± 3.5

11.6 ± 2.4

6.4 ± 2.9

X2=21.4; p=.0001 a

SCR Total

32.1 ± 14.7

16.5 ± 15.0

20.2 ± 16.0

F(2,42) = 4.34, p=.02

5.3 ± 2.3

3.1 ± 2.0

4.4 ± 2.0

F(2,44)=4.30, p=.02

CAPS Total

Subjective stress

NIH-PA Author Manuscript

a

independent samples Kruskal-Wallis non-parametric test.

NIH-PA Author Manuscript J Trauma Treat. Author manuscript; available in PMC 2014 November 05.

Skin conductance response during laboratory stress in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder.

The primary objective was to assess skin conductance response (SCR) to a laboratory stressor in combat veterans with posttraumatic stress disorder (PT...
152KB Sizes 1 Downloads 10 Views