Shared Decision Making Shared Decision Making State of the Science Grace A. Lin, MD, MAS; Angela Fagerlin, PhD

P

atients have traditionally entrusted decision making to physicians. However, during the past several decades, patients have been encouraged to become more activated and involved in their health decisions. These situations abound in cardiology, for example, therapy for stable coronary artery disease, anticoagulation for atrial fibrillation, and placement of implantable cardioverter-defibrillators. Each condition demands patient participation in the decision-making process because patients live with the consequences of medical decisions in their day-to-day lives. Unfortunately, research has also shown that patients often are misinformed about the risks and benefits associated with treatments and have little involvement in the decision-making process.1,2 Thus, 1 of the great challenges of increasing patient engagement is ensuring that patients make informed, evidence-based decisions that are consistent with their values and preferences. Shared decision making (SDM) has come to the forefront as a way to improve clinical care for patients by encouraging the production and dissemination of accurate, balanced, understandable health information and increasing patient participation in care. SDM interventions have been shown to improve patients’ understanding of the available treatment options, increase the proportion of patients with realistic expectations of benefits and harms, stimulate patients’ involvement in decision making, and improve agreement between patients’ values and treatment choices.3 Incorporating patient preferences into the decision-making process may also lead to improved patient well-being through better adherence to treatment, fewer concerns about illness, and higher satisfaction with health outcomes.4,5 In this article, we review the state of the science in the field of SDM. We discuss models of SDM, as well as methods for providing decision support to patients, including best practices for risk communication, efficacy of decision aids (DAs) for decision support, and use of decision coaches to facilitate shared decision making.

Models of SDM SDM was first invoked by the President’s Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research in 1982.6 One of the earliest and most commonly cited models of SDM is from Charles et al,7,8 aptly

subtitled “It Takes at Least Two to Tango.” The model lays out the basic principles of SDM (Table 1). There must be involvement of ≥2 participants, and each party takes steps to be involved in the decision-making process and freely share information with the other. The physician’s responsibility is to give accurate medical information, elicit and acknowledge patients’ preferences for participation, give the patients choices about how the decision-making process will proceed, and respect patients’ choices. Patients have the responsibility to communicate their values, goals, and preferences to the physician. Finally, when a treatment decision is made, both parties agree to the decision. Later models build on the basic principles of SDM laid out by Charles et al.7,8 A systematic review found significant variation in the definition of SDM in the literature, in both the processes documented and the roles and responsibilities of the patients and clinicians.9 Makoul and Clayman9 found that 4 models were most commonly cited,7,10–12 and they derived 9 essential process elements as follows: (1) defining and explaining the problem, (2) presenting options, (3) discussing pros and cons (benefits/ risks/costs), (4) sharing patient values and preferences, (5) discussing patient ability and self-efficacy, (6) offering doctor knowledge and communication, (7) checking and clarifying understanding, (8) making or explicitly deferring a decision, and (9) arranging follow-up. Other elements of SDM such as providing unbiased information, defining roles and desire for involvement, deliberation and negotiation, partnership, and coming to a mutual agreement are also mentioned. Although the principles underlying SDM have been clearly laid out, implementation of these principles into routine clinical care has been much less successful. Recognizing the practical barriers to implementation, more recent models have focused on how to involve patients effectively in the decision-­making process. For example, Elwyn et al13 have proposed a 3-step model designed to help patients through the decision-making process as follows: (1) introducing choice through offering options and justifications for those options, (2) describing options, and (3) helping patients explore preferences and make decisions. Matthias et al14 propose a model that emphasizes the importance of physician communication skills and emphasize that SDM is important not only at the decision point, which is the focus of most SDM interventions,

From the Department of Medicine and Philip R. Lee Institute for Health Policy Studies, University of California, San Francisco, CA (G.A.L.); Department of Internal Medicine and Center for Bioethics and Social Sciences in Medicine, University of Michigan, Ann Arbor, MI (A.F.); and Ann Arbor VA HSR&D Center for Practice Management and Outcomes Research, Ann Arbor, MI (A.F.). Correspondence to Grace A. Lin, MD, Philip R. Lee Institute for Health Policy Studies, 3333 California St, Suite 265, San Francisco, CA 94118. E-mail [email protected] (Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2014;7:328-334.) © 2014 American Heart Association, Inc. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes is available at http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org

DOI: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.113.000322

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015 328

Lin and Fagerlin   Science of Shared Decision Making   329 Table 1.  Comparison Between Types of Decision Making Stage of Decision Making

Paternalistic

In Between Approaches

Shared

In Between Approaches

Informed

Information exchange

One way, from provider to patient; provider decides what information to share

Two way; both parties share all relevant information (medical and personal)

One way, from provider to patient; provider shares all relevant information

Deliberation

Mainly provider

Physician and patient shared

Mainly patient

Decision

Provider alone

Provider led and patient opinion sought/offered; patient acknowledges and agrees

Physician and patient shared

Patient led and provider opinion sought/offered; provider acknowledges and agrees

Patient alone

Adapted with permission from Charles et al8 and Makoul and Clayman.9 Authorization for this adaptation has been obtained both from the owner of the copyright in the original work and from the owner of copyright in the translation or adaptation.

but during the entire encounter. However, there have been few studies of interventions to train healthcare professionals and increase adoption of SDM, so consistently effective programs for training in SDM have yet to be identified.15 Although most models of SDM concentrate on the relationship between an individual clinician and patient, Légaré et al16 have developed an approach that incorporates other actors, including family, surrogate decision makers, and an interprofessional team (physicians, nurses, health coaches, etc), and promotes collaboration among all parties to assist the patient in making a decision. The importance of such expanded models is likely to increase because care models increasingly shift toward use of interprofessional teams. Although the emphasis varies among the models of SDM discussed above, all describe ideal elements and processes to achieve SDM in clinical care (Table 2). Implementation of SDM requires both the recognition of preference-­sensitive decisions and the ability to provide decision support to patients. Several methods of providing decision support have been developed, the most common of which—DAs, health communication skills, and decision coaching—we describe below.

Providing Decision Support Most models of SDM are applicable to preference-sensitive decisions, in which there is >1 medically reasonable choice available, and the risk–benefit ratio depends on how the patient values certain outcomes (eg, Class IIa and IIb recommendations in clinical practice guidelines). For example, in patients with atrial fibrillation who qualify for anticoagulation therapy, the benefit of stroke prevention must be balanced against the risk of bleeding and the inconveniences associated with anticoagulant medications. In such situations, the information the patient provides about his or her preferences is critical to making the right decision. Unfortunately, physicians can often misdiagnose patient preferences if preferences are not explicitly discussed.17 However, SDM is also a continuum.18 Although most patients would like to be informed about a decision, their preferences for participation in the decision-making process may vary depending on the situation. For example, older patients tend to want to be informed of their choices but prefer to rely on physician recommendations.19 Women, more educated

patients, and patients deciding whether to undergo an invasive procedure tend toward wanting to be partners in the decision-­making process with the physician.19,20 On the contrary, patients facing uncertain or unfamiliar situations may need assistance from their physicians or other providers, decision coaches, or family members to form their preferences for participation and for treatment.21 Assessing the patient’s preference for style of decision making is a critical initial step toward identifying whether a shared decision-making process is appropriate for the situation. A common measure of preference for SDM is the Control Preference Scale, which asks people whether they prefer (1) to make the final treatment decision, (2) to make the final selection of their treatment after considering their doctor’s opinion, (3) that their doctor share responsibility, (4) that their doctor make the final decision after considering their opinion, or (5) to leave all treatment decisions to their doctor. This question could easily be asked in clinic visits, although it is only currently asked in research settings.22 Although many advocate SDM in theory, there are challenges for practicing SDM in the clinical setting. Physicians may feel that they do not have the time or skills to communicate patient-specific information about treatment options and outcomes, although studies show that engaging patients in SDM does not seem to lengthen the encounter.23,24 In the next section, we discuss evidence-based tools to help facilitate SDM.

Best Practices for Risk Communication A key piece of the SDM process is ensuring that patients are well informed about the risks, benefits, and alternatives of the treatment(s) available. Unfortunately, patients face numerous challenges in understanding the medical information they need to make informed decisions. Many of the available decision support materials are written at higher literacy levels than the average patient is able to read.25 The average American reads at about the eighth-grade reading level,26 yet DAs have been found to be written at a 12th-grade or higher level—meaning that the information is incomprehensible to many patients. Inadequate numeracy may also harm patients’ ability to make informed decisions. People who have difficulty understanding or using numeric information (eg, risk and benefit statistics) may have a reduced ability to make accurate comparisons between the risks and benefits of treatment (or no

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

330   Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes   March 2014 Table 2.  Characteristics of Models of Shared Decision Making Model

Context

Charles et al

Treatment decision making

7,8

Key SDM Characteristics Both physician and patient:  Are involved in the decision-making process  Share information with each other  Take steps to participate in the decision-making process by expressing treatment preferences  Agree on treatment to implement

Coulter10

Treatment decision making

Patient plays an equal part in both the process of decision making and the decision itself Physician supplies information about diagnosis, prognosis, and treatment options Patient clarifies preference for level of involvement and treatment preferences

Towle and Godolphin

12

Competencies for teaching, learning, practice, and research of SDM

Develop partnership with patient Establish patient’s preferences for information, role in decision making, and their ideas, concerns, expectations Identify choices and present information to patients Negotiate decision in partnership with patient Agree on plan

Elwyn et al13

Treatment decision making in primary care

Implicit or explicit involvement of patients in decision-making process Explore ideas, fears, expectations of the clinical problem, and treatments and portrayal of equipoise and options Provide tailored information and check patient understanding Involve patient in the decision-making process to the extent desired Make, discuss, or defer decision, and arrange follow-up

Makoul and Clayman (2006)9

Integrative model of treatment decision making based on literature review

Defines 9 essential elements of SDM, including:  Discussion of problem, options, benefits, risks, costs, patient values and preferences, and patient ability to follow treatment plan  Physician sharing knowledge and recommendations  Check and clarify understanding  Make or explicitly defer decision and arrange follow-up  Also discuss ideal elements of SDM

Légaré et al16

Integrating interprofessional approach to SDM

At least 2 healthcare professionals in different professions collaborate to achieve SDM with the patient, either concurrently or sequentially, including information exchange, clarification of values and preferences, discussion of feasibility of options, and discussion of preferred choice Discussion of influences from healthcare system on SDM, particularly environmental influences (eg, policies, social context, culture, resources)

SDM indicates shared decision making.

treatment). Innumeracy is pervasive, with approximately half of the adults in the United States unable to accurately calculate a tip26 and 20% of college-educated adults not knowing which of the following risks is higher: 1%, 5%, or 10%.27 To put the issues of low numeracy into clinical perspective, when a cardiologist tells a patient that 64% of patients with angina will have symptom relief with medical therapy in the first year or that the risk of heart attack during percutaneous coronary intervention is 2% to 5%, many patients will not understand such statistics well enough to use them as part of making an informed decision. Thus, patients may make decisions not reflective of their preferences. Finally, in part, because of patients’ difficulty with health literacy and numeracy, physicians often use verbal qualifiers (eg, you have a high risk of a heart attack) rather than using numeric or graphical formats. Although patients may prefer qualitative explanations of risk, studies have demonstrated that interpretation of these imprecise terms is highly variable.28 For example, high risk to a physician may mean 5%, but to a

patient, it may mean 25% (or vice versa). Thus, numeric and graphical representations of risk are preferred for clear risk communication. Below, we present 3 evidence-based tips on how to more clearly communicate risk to patients. Other tips, although geared toward oncologists, can be found elsewhere.29 Tip 1: Present Statistical Information Using Absolute Risk When trying to educate patients about the risks associated with their treatment options, one must decide whether to present that information using absolute or relative risk presentation. For example, a cardiologist could tell patients that taking a statin will decrease their risk of heart attack by 30% (relative risk) or that their risk of heart attack decreases from 10% to 7% (absolute risk). Research has consistently shown that changes in risk seem larger when presented using relative risk than when using an absolute risk30,31 and that medical treatments were viewed more favorably when presented in relative risk.32 Thus, the use of relative risk information can have negative consequences for patients in that it could cause patients to

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

Lin and Fagerlin   Science of Shared Decision Making   331 inaccurately perceive that a treatment is more effective than what has been empirically proven. Even physicians are unduly influenced by relative risk presentation. Chao et al33 had oncologists indicate their hypothetical prescribing behavior after reading a journal article describing a clinical trial of chemotherapy. Oncologists either had the benefits of chemotherapy presented using a relative risk or an absolute risk presentation. Oncologists were significantly more likely to prescribe the chemotherapy when the data were presented using relative risk information. An even more difficult risk presentation strategy for patients to understand is number needed to treat. Sheridan et al33a tested patients’ understanding of data presented using absolute risk, relative risk, and number needed to treat and found that number needed to treat was the most difficult risk communication method for patients to understand and recommend that it never be the only way risk information be communicated to patients. Tip 2: Highlight the Incremental Risks Associated With Treatment Distinctly From Baseline Risks Patients may not be able to differentiate their total risk of a side effect from the risk associated with treatment. For example, all adults have a baseline risk of stomach bleeding. However, that risk increases with taking aspirin. If patients just see the total risk of having stomach bleeding by those who take aspirin, they may perceive the risk of taking aspirin to be greater than what it is (ie, they may think that the entire risk is because of the drug because they are not accounting for baseline risks). Using an incremental risk approach can help differentiate these risks.34,35 For example, as seen in the Figure, the incremental risk approach draws attention to the change in risk. The risk of severe bleeding with aspirin (0.5 patients of 100) and the additional risk incurred if choosing warfarin (3.5 patients of 100) are depicted in the same pictograph. This

makes it clear how a person’s risk changes as a function of the treatment chosen. In a study of women at high risk of breast cancer who were considering chemoprevention, women who received risk information in a DA using the incremental risk approach were less worried about the side effects of treatment and perceived less risk of experiencing a side effect than those patients who received the same data but presented using a total risk presentation.34,35 This may be because of the fact that patients who only see the total risk may inaccurately think that all the risk is because of treatment and may not understand that some of the risk is baseline risk. The incremental risk approach addresses this inaccuracy and reduces this misperception. An important caveat is that the incremental risk approach was only beneficial when the data were also presented within a pictograph. Tip 3: Use Pictographs to Communicate Risk and Benefit Information A growing body of research has revealed that pictographs help all patients (regardless of numeracy) better comprehend risk information.36–38 Pictographs have been found to be more effective than bar graphs and tables and equally effective to pie graphs when communicating gist-level information (ie, the main point of the communication rather than the specific statistical information).36 Similarly, when the goal of communication is to convey exact statistical information, pictographs were again one of the top graphic formats as they were equally effective as bar graphs and tables but more effective than pie graphs.36 Thus, for communicating gist or verbatim information, pictographs were among the most effective graphical formats. However, recent articles suggest that when numbers are presented within graphs, bar graphs and pictographs have produced similar knowledge.39 Finally,

Figure. Pictograph (icon array) depicting incremental risk. The pictograph displays the incremental risk of anticoagulation for atrial fibrillation: if aspirin is chosen for anticoagulation, 0.5 patients of 100 will experience a major bleeding episode; if warfarin is chosen, 3.5 additional patients of 100 will experience a major bleeding episode compared with choosing aspirin (pictograph generated at iconarray.com).

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

332   Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes   March 2014 pictographs have been successfully incorporated in several DAs and intervention studies.40,41 Efficacy of DAs for Decision Support for Cardiovascular Diseases DAs are materials containing evidence-based information designed to assist patients in making decisions that involve weighing benefits and harms. They differ from patient educational materials in that they also help patients clarify their values and better communicate and share decision making with their physicians.3 Multiple DAs have been developed to address common decisions in cardiology (Table 3). Some tools are designed for face-to-face encounters to provide a framework for discussion. For example, the Mayo Clinic’s Statin Choice DA is designed for use during an encounter to assist physicians and patients to collaboratively discuss the risks and benefits of statin therapy based on patients’ personal risk of coronary artery disease. Other DAs have been developed for use as an adjunct to a physician visit. Such tools tend to contain comprehensive information about a clinical problem and treatment options and often contain information to help patients clarify their values and preferences. For example, video-based DAs are available from Informed Medical Decisions Foundation for patients with stable coronary artery disease. DAs for invasive procedures such as implantable cardioverter-defibrillators and percutaneous coronary intervention are currently under development (D. Matlock, MD, MPH, unpublished data, 2013).42 Evidence of efficacy of DAs addressing cardiology issues is mixed. Randomized, controlled trials of DAs for coronary artery disease treatment, statin use, anticoagulation for atrial fibrillation, heart failure self-management, vascular access for cardiac catheterization, acute low-risk chest pain, and coronary artery disease prevention have shown that use of such DAs improves the decision-making process by increasing knowledge, improving risk communication, decreasing decisional conflict, increasing participation in care, and increasing value congruence.43–48 The effect of DAs on such outcomes as self-efficacy and medication adherence has not yet been definitively established.

Veroff et al46 found that heart failure patients randomized to receive a DA were significantly more likely to weigh themselves daily, monitor fluid intake, and limit salt intake than those not receiving a DA. However, there was only short-­term follow-up (4 weeks). Effects on medication adherence also have been mixed. For example, randomized, controlled trials of the statin choice DAs have shown an increase in accuracy of the patients’ risk perception but variable effects on medication adherence.45,49 Medication adherence may be affected by other factors than understanding of risks and benefits, so one exposure to a DA may not be sufficient to change behavior. Whether DAs can—or should—affect outcomes beyond the decisionmaking process needs to be addressed in future studies.

Use of Decision Coaching to Facilitate SDM Although DAs have numerous benefits, the literature is mixed with regard to whether DAs alone change treatment preferences.50 This may be because of patients having difficulty identifying what they do not understand or thinking through how their values and goals should inform their decision making. Decision coaches may help patients make more informed decisions by helping the patient to determine what questions they have and to think about the goals and values they have that relate to the medical decision. Decision coaching that is used to support decision making aims to provide patients with education resources (eg, DAs), help patients formulate questions related to the decision, and encourage patients to consider their values and goals of treatment.51,52 For example, Belkora et al52 have successfully used a decision coaching program to improve decision making in breast cancer patients by having patients review a DA and then participate in a structured interview with a trained coach. Topics covered during the consultation planning included the following: (1) questions or concerns about their diagnosis, (2) treatment options available, (3) objectives and goals for consultation and treatment, (4) key people involved in decision, and (5) the kind of information patients want or need to make the decision. The coach recorded the patients’ questions, concerns, and values into a document that was given to the

Table 3.  Useful Websites for Decision Aids Organization

Website

Description

Cardiology-Focused Decision Aids

Ottawa Hospital Research Institute

http://www.decisionaid.ohri.ca

Comprehensive list of available decision aids, toolkits for implementing decision support

Angina, AF, CAD, hyperlipidemia, stroke, valvular diseases

Mayo Clinic Knowledge and Evaluation Unit

http://shareddecisions.mayoclinic.org

Paper and electronic decision aids for in-visit use

Statin for hyperlipidemia

Option Grids

www.optiongrid.org

Paper decision aids for in-visit use

Angina, AF

MED-DECS

http://www.med-decs.org/

Compilation of online decision aids

Cardiovascular disease prevention, AF, hypertension, hyperlipidemia

Health Dialog/Informed Medical Decisions Foundation

http://imdfoundation.org

Video-based decision aids

CAD, CHF

http://www.healthcrossroads.com

Healthwise

http://www.healthwise.org

Online interactive decision aids

AF, pacemaker, implantable cardioverterdefibrillator, supraventricular tachycardia, hyperlipidemia, coronary artery bypass surgery, angioplasty, CHF, aortic stenosis, valve replacement, hypertension

AF indicates atrial fibrillation; CAD, coronary artery disease; and CHF, congestive heart failure.

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

Lin and Fagerlin   Science of Shared Decision Making   333 patients and clinicians. Finally, the decision coach also accompanied patients to the clinic visit to take notes for the patients. This process has been tested in several studies with positive results.51,53 Patients who participated in coaching had higher decision quality, and there was a greater match between patient and physician perceptions of the quality of the decision-­making process. In addition, patients reported greater satisfaction with the appointment. The note taking also had significant benefits because patients did not have to worry about remembering the entire discussion, and thus the quality of the consultation improved. There were also benefits for the physicians.53 Significantly, neither the length of appointments nor the number of ­follow-up appointments differed between those who received or did not receive coaching. In fact, patients and physicians were able to accomplish more in the same amount of time, and, anecdotally, physicians felt that the intervention helped them achieve a more effective, higher-level conversation that was focused on the critical issues. Note taking by the decision coach also led to physicians having greater confidence that patients had accurate recollections of the discussion.51 Other researchers have developed different decision coaching programs that provide patients with a list of suggested questions to ask. Studies have found that these lists increased the number and quality of questions asked at medical appointments.54 Furthermore, some have suggested that 1 of the best methods for improving chronic disease care is to improve patient–physician communication by helping patients better prepare for their clinical encounter. Patients who feel that their physician understands their concerns, for example, feel more in control of their medication regimen, which could significantly improve patient health.55 Finally, decision coaching that incorporates the use of DAs improves decision quality, increases patient satisfaction, and is cost effective.54 Trials of decision coaching for cardiovascular conditions, testing either individual coaching or group coaching led by primarily trained nurse coaches, have demonstrated that decision coaching seems to improve patients’ knowledge, level of anxiety, ability to participate in SDM with providers, and satisfaction with communication.56,57 However, as with DAs, evidence that such interventions affect health outcomes has not yet been established.

SDM Now and in the Future SDM is 1 method of meeting the Institute of Medicine’s goal of providing patient-centered care. Practicing SDM with patients, particularly using DAs, clear risk communication, and decision coaching, can lead to improvements in patient knowledge, more accurate perceptions of risk and benefit, as well as increasing patient participation in the decision-making process and increasing the concordance between patient values and the treatment decision. The push to include SDM in clinical care and indeed make it standard of care is increasing, with both state and federal legislation recognizing that patient participation in healthcare decisions is critical to building a high-quality healthcare delivery system. Physicians can and should lead the way of incorporating SDM into their practice, and future installments of this series will provide guidance for successful implementation.

Acknowledgments We thank Mark Dickson and Brian Zikmund-Fisher, PhD, for their assistance in creating the pictograph.

Disclosures Dr Lin has received grant funding from the Informed Medical Decisions Foundation. Dr Fagerlin has received an honorarium from Genentech.

References 1. Hauptman PJ, Chibnall JT, Guild C, Armbrecht ES. Patient perceptions, physician communication, and the implantable cardioverter-defibrillator. JAMA Intern Med. 2013;173:571–577. 2. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Couper MP, Singer E, Ubel PA, Ziniel S, Fowler FJ Jr, Levin CA, Fagerlin A. Deficits and variations in patients’ experience with making 9 common medical decisions: the DECISIONS survey. Med Decis Making. 2010;30(5 suppl):85S–95S. 3. Stacey D, Bennett CL, Barry MJ, Col NF, Eden KB, Holmes-Rovner M, Llewellyn-Thomas H, Lyddiatt A, Legare F, Thomson R. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2011;10:CD001431. 4. Greenfield S, Kaplan SH, Ware JE Jr, Yano EM, Frank HJ. Patients’ participation in medical care: effects on blood sugar control and quality of life in diabetes. J Gen Intern Med. 1988;3:448–457. 5. Kaplan SH, Greenfield S, Ware JE Jr. Assessing the effects of physician-­ patient interactions on the outcomes of chronic disease. Med Care. 1989;27(3 suppl):S110–S127. 6. Presidents' Commission for the Study of Ethical Problems in Medicine and Biomedical and Behavioral Research. Making health care decisions: the ethical and legal implications of informed consent in the patientpractitioner relationship. U S Code Annot U S. 1982;42:300v. 7. Charles C, Gafni A, Whelan T. Shared decision-making in the medical encounter: what does it mean? (or it takes at least two to tango). Soc Sci Med. 1997;44:681–692. 8. Charles C, Gafni A, Whelan T. Decision-making in the physician-­patient encounter: revisiting the shared treatment decision-making model. Soc Sci Med. 1999;49:651–661. 9. Makoul G, Clayman ML. An integrative model of shared decision making in medical encounters. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;60:301–312. 10. Coulter A. Partnerships with patients: the pros and cons of shared clinical decision-making. J Health Serv Res Policy. 1997;2:112–121. 11. Elwyn G, Edwards A, Kinnersley P, Grol R. Shared decision making and the concept of equipoise: the competences of involving patients in healthcare choices. Br J Gen Pract. 2000;50:892–899. 12. Towle A, Godolphin W. Framework for teaching and learning informed shared decision making. BMJ. 1999;319:766–771. 13. Elwyn G, Frosch D, Thomson R, Joseph-Williams N, Lloyd A, Kinnersley P, Cording E, Tomson D, Dodd C, Rollnick S, Edwards A, Barry M. Shared decision making: a model for clinical practice. J Gen Intern Med. 2012;27:1361–1367. 14. Matthias MS, Salyers MP, Frankel RM. Re-thinking shared decision-­ making: context matters. Patient Educ Couns. 2013;91:176–179. 15. Legare F, Ratte S, Stacey D, Kryworuchko J, Gravel K, Graham ID, Turcotte S. Interventions for improving the adoption of shared decision making by healthcare professionals. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2010:CD006732. 16. Légaré F, Stacey D, Pouliot S, Gauvin FP, Desroches S, Kryworuchko J, Dunn S, Elwyn G, Frosch D, Gagnon MP, Harrison MB, Pluye P, Graham ID. Interprofessionalism and shared decision-making in primary care: a stepwise approach towards a new model. J Interprof Care. 2011;25:18–25. 17. Mulley AG, Trimble C, Elwyn G. Stop the silent misdiagnosis: patients’ preferences matter. BMJ. 2012;345:e6572. 18. Kon AA. The shared decision-making continuum. JAMA. 2010;304:903–904. 19. Levinson W, Kao A, Kuby A, Thisted RA. Not all patients want to participate in decision making: a national study of public preferences. J Gen Intern Med. 2005;20:531–535. 20. Chewning B, Bylund CL, Shah B, Arora NK, Gueguen JA, Makoul G. Patient preferences for shared decisions: a systematic review. Patient Educ Couns. 2012;86:9–18. 21. Epstein RM, Peters E. Beyond information: exploring patients’ preferences. JAMA. 2009;302:195–197. 22. Degner LF, Sloan JA, Venkatesh P. The Control Preferences Scale. Can J Nurs Res. 1997;29:21–43.

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

334   Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes   March 2014 23. Légaré F, Ratté S, Gravel K, Graham ID. Barriers and facilitators to implementing shared decision-making in clinical practice: update of a systematic review of health professionals’ perceptions. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73:526–535. 24. Edwards A, Elwyn G, Wood F, Atwell C, Prior L, Houston H. Shared decision making and risk communication in practice: a qualitative study of GPs’ experiences. Br J Gen Pract. 2005;55:6–13. 25. Fagerlin A, Rovner D, Stableford S, Jentoft C, Wei JT, Holmes-Rovner M. Patient education materials about the treatment of early-stage prostate cancer: a critical review. Ann Intern Med. 2004;140:721–728. 26. Kirsch IS, Jungeblut A, Jenkins L, Kolstad A. Adult Literacy in America. A First Look at the Results of the National Adult Literacy Survey. Washington, D.C.: National Center for Education Statistics, 1993. 27. Lipkus IM, Samsa G, Rimer BK. General performance on a numeracy scale among highly educated samples. Med Decis Making. 2001;21:37–44. 28. Burkell J. What are the chances? Evaluating risk and benefit information in consumer health materials. J Med Libr Assoc. 2004;92:200–208. 29. Fagerlin A, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Ubel PA. Helping patients decide: ten steps to better risk communication. J Natl Cancer Inst. 2011;103:1436–1443. 30. Malenka DJ, Baron JA, Johansen S, Wahrenberger JW, Ross JM. The framing effect of relative and absolute risk. J Gen Intern Med. 1993;8:543–548. 31. Forrow L, Taylor WC, Arnold RM. Absolutely relative: how research results are summarized can affect treatment decisions. Am J Med. 1992;92:121–124. 32. Covey J. A meta-analysis of the effects of presenting treatment benefits in different formats. Med Decis Making. 2007;27:638–654. 33. Chao C, Studts JL, Abell T, Hadley T, Roetzer L, Dineen S, Lorenz D, YoussefAgha A, McMasters KM. Adjuvant chemotherapy for breast cancer: how presentation of recurrence risk influences decision-making. J Clin Oncol. 2003;21:4299–4305. 33a. Sheridan SL, Pignone MP, Lewis CL. A randomized comparison of patients' understanding of number needed to treat and other common risk reduction formats. J Gen Intern Med. 2003; 18: 884–892. 34. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Ubel PA, Smith DM, Derry HA, McClure JB, Stark A, Pitsch RK, Fagerlin A. Communicating side effect risks in a tamoxifen prophylaxis decision aid: the debiasing influence of pictographs. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73:209–214. 35. Zikmund-Fisher B, Fagerlin A, Roberts T, Derry H, Ubel P. Alternate methods of framing information about medication side effects: incremental risk versus total risk occurence. J Health Commun. 2008;13:107–124. 36. Hawley ST, Zikmund-Fisher B, Ubel P, Jancovic A, Lucas T, Fagerlin A. The impact of the format of graphical presentation on health-­related knowledge and treatment choices. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73:448–455. 37. Garcia-Retamero R, Galesic M, Gigerenzer G. Do icon arrays help reduce denominator neglect? Med Decis Making. 2010;30:672–684. 38. Tait AR, Voepel-Lewis T, Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Fagerlin A. The effect of format on parents’ understanding of the risks and benefits of clinical research: a comparison between text, tables, and graphics. J Health Commun. 2010;15:487–501. 39. Tait AR, Voepel-Lewis T, Brennan-Martinez C, McGonegal M, Levine R. Using animated computer-generated text and graphics to depict the risks and benefits of medical treatment. Am J Med. 2012;125:1103–1110. 40. Zikmund-Fisher BJ, Ubel PA, Smith DM, Derry HA, McClure JB, Stark A, Pitsch RK, Fagerlin A. Communicating side effect risks in a tamoxifen prophylaxis decision aid: the debiasing influence of pictographs. Patient Educ Couns. 2008;73:209–214. 41. Holmes-Rovner M, Stableford S, Fagerlin A, Wei JT, Dunn RL, Ohene-­ Frempong J, Kelly-Blake K, Rovner DR. Evidence-based patient choice: a prostate cancer decision aid in plain language. BMC Med Inform Decis Mak. 2005;5:16.

42. Coylewright M, Shepel K, Leblanc A, Pencille L, Hess E, Shah N, Montori VM, Ting HH. Shared decision making in patients with stable coronary artery disease: PCI choice. PLoS One. 2012;7:e49827. 43. Morgan MW, Deber RB, Llewellyn-Thomas HA, Gladstone P, Cusimano RJ, O’Rourke K, Tomlinson G, Detsky AS. Randomized, controlled trial of an interactive videodisc decision aid for patients with ischemic heart disease. J Gen Intern Med. 2000;15:685–693. 44. Liao L, Jollis JG, DeLong ER, Peterson ED, Morris KG, Mark DB. Impact of an interactive video on decision making of patients with ischemic heart disease. J Gen Intern Med. 1996;11:373–376. 45. Mann DM, Ponieman D, Montori VM, Arciniega J, McGinn T. The Statin Choice decision aid in primary care: a randomized trial. Patient Educ Couns. 2010;80:138–140. 46. Veroff DR, Sullivan LA, Shoptaw EJ, Venator B, Ochoa-Arvelo T, Baxter JR, Manocchia M, Wennberg D. Improving self-care for heart failure for seniors: the impact of video and written education and decision aids. Popul Health Manag. 2012;15:37–45. 47. Schwalm JD, Stacey D, Pericak D, Natarajan MK. Radial artery versus femoral artery access options in coronary angiogram procedures: randomized controlled trial of a patient-decision aid. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2012;5:260–266. 48. Hess EP, Knoedler MA, Shah ND, Kline JA, Breslin M, Branda ME, Pencille LJ, Asplin BR, Nestler DM, Sadosty AT, Stiell IG, Ting HH, Montori VM. The chest pain choice decision aid: a randomized trial. Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2012;5:251–259. 49. Weymiller AJ, Montori VM, Jones LA, Gafni A, Guyatt GH, Bryant SC, Christianson TJ, Mullan RJ, Smith SA. Helping patients with type 2 diabetes mellitus make treatment decisions: statin choice randomized trial. Arch Intern Med. 2007;167:1076–1082. 50. O’Connor AM, Bennett CL, Stacey D, Barry M, Col NF, Eden KB, Entwiistle VA, Fiset V, Holmes-Rovner M, Khangura S, Llewellyn-­ Thomas H, Rovner D. Decision aids for people facing health treatment or screening decisions. Cochrane Database Syst Rev. 2009;3:CD001431. 51. Sepucha KR, Belkora JK, Tripathy D, Esserman LJ. Building bridges between physicians and patients: results of a pilot study examining new tools for collaborative decision making in breast cancer. J Clin Oncol. 2000;18:1230–1238. 52. Belkora J, Katapodi M, Moore D, Franklin L, Hopper K, Esserman L. Evaluation of a visit preparation intervention implemented in two rural, underserved counties of Northern California. Patient Educ Couns. 2006;64:350–359. 53. Belkora J, Edlow B, Aviv C, Sepucha K, Esserman L. Training community resource center and clinic personnel to prompt patients in listing questions for doctors: follow-up interviews about barriers and facilitators to the implementation of consultation planning. Implement Sci. 2008;3:6. 54. Stacey D, Samant R, Bennett C. Decision making in oncology: a review of patient decision aids to support patient participation. CA Cancer J Clin. 2008;58:293–304. 55. Thorne SE, Paterson BL. Health care professional support for self-care management in chronic illness: insights from diabetes research. Patient Educ Couns. 2001;42:81–90. 56. Koelewijn-van Loon MS, van der Weijden T, Ronda G, van Steenkiste B, Winkens B, Elwyn G, Grol R. Improving lifestyle and risk perception through patient involvement in nurse-led cardiovascular risk management: a cluster-randomized controlled trial in primary care. Prev Med. 2010;50:35–44. 57. Dontje K, Kelly-Blake K, Olomu A, Rothert M, Dwamena F, Henry R, Rovner D, Holmes-Rovner M. Nurse-led group visits support shared decision making in stable coronary artery disease. J Cardiovasc Nurs. 2013;28:269–276. Key Words:  shared decision-making

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

Shared Decision Making: State of the Science Grace A. Lin and Angela Fagerlin Circ Cardiovasc Qual Outcomes. 2014;7:328-334; originally published online February 4, 2014; doi: 10.1161/CIRCOUTCOMES.113.000322 Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes is published by the American Heart Association, 7272 Greenville Avenue, Dallas, TX 75231 Copyright © 2014 American Heart Association, Inc. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 1941-7705. Online ISSN: 1941-7713

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/content/7/2/328

Permissions: Requests for permissions to reproduce figures, tables, or portions of articles originally published in Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes can be obtained via RightsLink, a service of the Copyright Clearance Center, not the Editorial Office. Once the online version of the published article for which permission is being requested is located, click Request Permissions in the middle column of the Web page under Services. Further information about this process is available in the Permissions and Rights Question and Answer document. Reprints: Information about reprints can be found online at: http://www.lww.com/reprints Subscriptions: Information about subscribing to Circulation: Cardiovascular Quality and Outcomes is online at: http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org//subscriptions/

Downloaded from http://circoutcomes.ahajournals.org/ at University of Chicago on July 5, 2015

Shared decision making: state of the science.

Shared decision making: state of the science. - PDF Download Free
584KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views