Author's Accepted Manuscript

Plant Reproduction and Environmental Noise: How do Plants Do It? Danielle Lyles, Todd S. Rosenstock, Alan Hastings

www.elsevier.com/locate/yjtbi

PII: DOI: Reference:

S0022-5193(15)00068-5 http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.02.009 YJTBI8079

To appear in:

Journal of Theoretical Biology

Cite this article as: Danielle Lyles, Todd S. Rosenstock, Alan Hastings, Plant Reproduction and Environmental Noise: How do Plants Do It?, Journal of Theoretical Biology, http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.jtbi.2015.02.009 This is a PDF file of an unedited manuscript that has been accepted for publication. As a service to our customers we are providing this early version of the manuscript. The manuscript will undergo copyediting, typesetting, and review of the resulting galley proof before it is published in its final citable form. Please note that during the production process errors may be discovered which could affect the content, and all legal disclaimers that apply to the journal pertain.

Plant Reproduction and Environmental Noise: How Do Plants Do It? Danielle Lyles a,1,∗, Todd S. Rosenstockb,2 , Alan Hastingsc a

Department of Environmental Science and Policy, One Shields Avenue, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 b Department of Plant Sciences, One Shields Avenue, University of California, Davis, CA 95616 c Department of Environmental Science and Policy, One Shields Avenue, University of California, Davis, CA 95616

Abstract Plant populations exhibit a wide continuum of reproductive behavior, ranging from nearly constant reproductive output on one end to the extreme of masting (synchronized, highly variable reproduction) on the other. Here, we show that including variability (noise) in density-dependent pollen limitation in current models for pollen-limited plant reproduction may produce any behavior on this continuum. We previously showed that (large) variability in pollination efficiency (a related phenomenon) may induce masting in non-pollen-limited plant populations. Other modeling studies have shown that including variability in accumulated resources (and/or the threshold for reproduction) may induce masting, but do account for masting in non-pollen-limited plant populations. Thus, our results suggest that the range of plant reproductive behavior may be explained with the simple resource budget model combined with the biological realism of variability ∗

Corresponding author Email address: [email protected] (Danielle Lyles ) 1 Current address: Department of Mathematics, The University of Texas at San Antonio, San Antonio, TX 78249 2 Current address: World Agroforestry Centre (ICRAF), PO Box 30677-00100, Nairobi, Kenya

Preprint submitted to Journal of Theoretical Biology

February 11, 2015

in density-dependent pollen limitation. This is a specific example of an important functional consequence of the interactions between stochasticity and nonlinearity, and highlights the importance of carefully considering both the biological basis and the mathematical effects of the noise term. Keywords: moran, masting, synchronized, hybrid, model

1

1. Introduction

2

Much of the development of the understanding of population dynamics in

3

ecology has come from models that are deterministic (Kingsland, 1995). How-

4

ever, as far back as the work of Moran (Moran, 1953) on synchrony, the impor-

5

tance of stochasticity, and in particular external environmental variability, in shap-

6

ing spatiotemporal dynamics has been recognized. More recently, in a variety of

7

areas there has been increasing attention paid to the importance of variability.

8

However, much of the stochastic theory has been based on linearizations, or on

9

assumptions that the level of noise is small (Lande et al., 2003). In contrast to this

10

theory it has been recognized that natural variability can be very large, due to envi-

11

ronmental cues such as highly variable rainfall patterns or timing of frosts, which

12

in turn produces highly variable population densities through time (Connell and

13

Sousa, 1983). Thus, it is important for ecologists to get an understanding of the

14

importance of large variability as an influence on spatiotemporal dynamics. Yet,

15

this understanding can only come from attention to specific systems, like masting

16

behavior of plants (Liebhold et al., 2004).

17

Masting is synchronized, highly variable reproduction in a population of plants.

18

Masting may be divided into three categories: strict (bimodal with no overlap be-

19

tween tails), normal bimodal (bimodal with overlap between tails, bimodality is

2

20

statistically significant), or normal switching (output varies greatly and the pres-

21

ence of switching is verified by negative autocorrelation of reproductive output)

22

(Kelly, 1994). Although masting has received much attention, there is no distinct

23

line that may be drawn between masting and non-masting species (Kelly, 1994).

24

Plants that truly mast should vary more than their environment (Kelly, 1994). The

25

predominant hypothesis for this, resource switching, suggests that reproduction

26

is carbon-limited and the individual plant alternates allocation of carbon between

27

reproductive and vegetative structures.

28

While resource switching and carbon allocation patterns might explain vari-

29

able reproduction,how might a population of plants synchronize their reproduc-

30

tion? Empirical studies have shown that weather cues may synchronize plants for

31

masting (Schauber et al., 2002; Kelly et al., 2000, 2008; Sel˙as, 2000; Kon et al.,

32

2005; Kon and Noda, 2007). Such studies also repeatedly find that both internal

33

resource dynamics (resource switching) and cues from appropriate weather vari-

34

ables (which are noisy) must be combined in order to explain masting data (Sel˙as,

35

2000; Kon et al., 2005; Rees et al., 2002; Lamontagne and Boutin, 2007). Further-

36

more, a meta-analysis of masting data on a global scale has shown that masting

37

species with higher variability generally are located in areas with higher variabil-

38

ity in rainfall (Kelly and Sork, 2002). Kelly et al. have also shown that, for a

39

given plant population, those at higher altitudes have lowered temperature thresh-

40

olds for a given flowering effort (Kelly et al., 2008), suggesting that the variability

41

of temperature is more important than absolute temperature.

42

Mathematical modeling studies support the idea that resource switching com-

43

bined with environmental noise is an important component of the mechanism of

44

masting in plant populations. Both Isagi (Isagi et al., 1997) and Satake & Iwasa

3

45

(Satake and Iwasa, 2000, 2002a) have shown that resource switching combined

46

with pollen limitation may cause masting in monoecious plants. Satake and Iwasa

47

have also shown that such a mechanism may be greatly enhanced by the addition

48

of environmental noise in the form of variability in accumulated resources and/or

49

reproductive thresholds of individual plants, but only in the presence of pollen lim-

50

itation (Satake and Iwasa, 2002b). Previously, we extended their model to show

51

that resource switching combined with (large) environmental noise in the form

52

of variability in pollination efficiency may cause masting in plant populations in

53

the absence of pollen limitation and over a wide range of conditions (Lyles et al.,

54

2009). These examples are similar to the Moran effect, whereby independent

55

(linear) populations are synchronized by spatially correlated environmental noise

56

over a large spatial scale.

57

Pollen limitation is common among plants (Obeso, 2002) and it’s statistical

58

significance varies among sites and years, so the pollination environment is not

59

constant (Burd, 1994). Here, we examine the effect of variability in density-

60

dependent pollen limitation on the reproductive output of pollen-limited plant

61

populations. This may also be viewed as variability in the effectiveness of abi-

62

otic pollination under different conditions. For example, suppose that pollination

63

is a process that has a certain probability of success p on each trial. The effective-

64

ness of pollination can be defined as the probability of successful pollination for

65

each trial. So if p is the probability of pollination in one trial, then the probability

66

of pollination after n trials is 1 − (1 − p)n . If the number of trials is proportional to

67

the number of flowering plants, then this is equivalent to density dependent pollen

68

limitation, and variation in p is analogous to variation in density-dependent pollen

69

limitation. Variability in density-dependent pollen limitation may arise in many

4

70

ways. For example, from a change in distance between partners (habitat frag-

71

mentation) (Aguilar et al., 2006) or asynchrony in flower development between

72

male and female flowers (Vankin et al. 2006). Synchronous flowering is neces-

73

sary to achieve high density-dependent pollen limitation and regional climate may

74

influence the timing of flowering among several species of plants (Post, 2003;

75

Schauber et al., 2002). Other aspects of the weather and its inherent variabil-

76

ity may also change density-dependent pollen limitation in some years especially

77

for wind-pollinated plants (Koenig and Ashley, 2003). For example, precipita-

78

tion during flowering may limit pollen dispersal and cause mechanical damage to

79

flowers (Knapp et al., 2001; Knops et al., 2007). Variability in density-dependent

80

pollen limitation may also reflect variability in the effectiveness of biotic pollina-

81

tion under different conditions, in the presence of pollinators that are not highly

82

specialized. For example, if the probability of transferring pollen among con-

83

specifics increases with the density of conspecifics. There is an active literature

84

in pollination ecology exploring this issue (Kunin, 1993; Crone, 2013). Thus,

85

density-dependent pollen limitation can vary, perhaps quite significantly, from

86

year-to-year. In this paper we investigate whether such noise in density-dependent

87

pollen limitation may contribute to masting behavior in the (pollen limited) plant

88

population model(s) of Satake and Iwasa (Satake and Iwasa, 2000, 2002a,b).

89

2. Modeling, Simulation, and Analysis

90

2.1. Model

91

The model we use extends our earlier work regarding plants that aren’t pollen-

92

limited (Lyles et al., 2009) by expanding upon the idea that large variability in

93

pollination efficiency (here represented by variability in density-dependent pollen 5

94

limitation) is an important element for determining the reproductive behavior of

95

plants. This work was based on the models by Satake and Iwasa (Satake and

96

Iwasa, 2000, 2002a,b; Iwasa and Satake, 2004) developed from a still earlier

97

model by Isagi et al. (Isagi et al., 1997). The models are developed in terms

98

of the resource budget for an individual plant which adds resources each year due

99

to photosynthesis.

100

Plants are assumed to flower if resources at the beginning of the year are above

101

a threshold level, and not to flower if resources are below the threshold. If plants

102

do flower, the amount of flowering depends on the difference between resources

103

available and the threshold level, and the amount of resources is reduced by an

104

amount depending on the fruit set. By nondimensionalization and rescaling, the

105

threshold level for flowering can be assumed to be zero, and in a deterministic

106

description the amount of resources added each year can be assumed to be one

107

(Satake and Iwasa, 2000). Letting y x (t) represent non-dimensionalized resource

108

stores of individual plant in a population (plant x) leads to the model:

if y x (t) < 0, y x (t + 1) = y x (t) + 1

(1)

if y x (t) > 0, y x (t + 1) = −k ∗ P(y x (t), β x (t)) ∗ y x (t) + 1

(2)

109

where k > 0 represents resource depletion due to reproduction (the cost of

110

reproduction), β x (t) is variable density-dependent pollen limitation, and the func-

111

112

tion P(y x (t), β x (t)) is a pollen limitation term that determines the contribution of  pollen from nearby plants. In particular, P(y x (t), β x (t)) = ( 1n i∈U x [yi (t)]+ )βx (t)

113

where [y]+ = y if y ≥ 0 and [y]+ = 0 if y ≤ 0 and U x contains individuals

114

within the pollen dispersal distance D of plant x and n is the number of plants in

115

U x . The term P(y x (t), β x (t)) is the average pollen contribution of trees within the 6

116

pollen dispersal distance D of tree x (a number in [0,1]) raised to the power β x (t),

117

and so has values in the interval [0,1]. When β x (t) = 0, there is no pollen limi-

118

tation. When β x (t) is small, small amounts of pollen are magnified so the effects

119

of pollen limitation are minimal. When β x (t) is large, it takes larger amounts of

120

pollen to produce a similar pollen contribution, implying strong pollen limitation.

121

The key feature of β x (t) is that the ratio of seeds to flowers depends on the density

122

of flowering plants. This density dependence provides an endogenous feedback

123

that promotes synchrony. The noise term β x (t) is normally distributed with mean

124

βavg and coefficient of variation cv, so the standard deviation is cvβavg . The cor-

125

relation is described by a parameter R by writing β x (t) as a combination of two

126

variables as follows (Satake and Iwasa, 2002b): β x (t) = δ(t) + γ x (t)

(3)

129

where δ(t) is normally distributed with mean Rβavg and standard deviation √ cvβavg R and γ x (t) is normally distributed with mean (1 − R)βavg and standard √ deviation cvβavg 1 − R. Thus, when R = 1, β x (t) is the same value for each in-

130

dividual and when R = 0, β x (t) is independent among individuals. Intermediate

131

levels of correlation are obtain for values of R between 0 and 1. We allow β x (t) to

132

be normally distributed to compare to the case of noise in the threshold for repro-

133

duction and/or annual net production (Satake and Iwasa, 2002b). Since negative

134

β x (t) ignore biological realities, we set negative β x (t) values equal to zero in the

135

simulations.

136

2.2. Simulation

127

128

137

Individual plants were represented as positions in a 10 X 10 rectangular array,

138

which for the purposes of this paper, may be referred to as a ”forest”. Our results 7

139

with a 10 x 10 lattice match those of Satake and Iwasa with a 100 x 100 lattice

140

(Satake and Iwasa, 2002a), suggesting our lattice size is sufficient. Simulations

141

were started from a random initial condition, uniformly distributed throughout the

142

interval of possible values and run for 100 years unless otherwise stated.

143

2.3. Analysis

144

In order to study masting behavior, we must quantify ”synchronized, highly

145

variable reproduction”. To quantify synchrony, we use a measure of total syn-

146

chrony, S Y NC (as we did in (Lyles et al., 2009), following Satake and Iwasa

147

(Satake and Iwasa, 2002a)): S Y NC =

Vby , Vby + Vwy

(4)

148

where Vby represents average between-year variance of the population (repro-

149

ductive output of model population averaged for each year and then the variance

150

computed between years) and Vwy represents average within year variance of the

151

population (variance between individual reproductive output computed for each

152

year and then averaged). If the within year variance becomes arbitrarily small rel-

153

ative to between year variance, S Y NC → 1 and the population is considered to be

154

synchronized. Conversely, as within-year variance increases relative to between-

155

year variance S Y NC → 0 representing a population which is not synchronized.

156

Mean S Y NC values were calculated by averaging 100 S Y NC values that were

157

each calculated after running the simulation for 100 years (averaging 1,000 S Y NC

158

values provided similar results.) We verified whether moderate to high S Y NC

159

values corresponded to masting behavior by checking time series output of the

160

model. This was true for all cases except the globally coupled system when k < 1

8

161

and β x (t) is constant, which exhibits a stable equilibrium (highly synchronized but

162

not variable).

163

3. Results

164

We define masting to be synchronized, variable reproduction whose variabil-

165

ity is nearly double that of the environment and with a coefficient of variation

166

(CV) of at least one-half: environmental variability should be amplified and vari-

167

ability should be fairly large. Other studies have required CV to be larger than 1

168

(Liebhold et al., 2004; Lamontagne and Boutin, 2007) or larger than 1.6 (Kelly,

169

1994), however this is more applicable to plants whose average reproductive out-

170

put is near zero because CV increases without bound as the mean approaches

171

zero. Some plants, for example pistachio, may have average output much higher

172

than zero, decreasing the CV, though they exhibit a similar degree of variabil-

173

ity and other characteristics of masting behavior, such as delayed reproduction

174

(Stevenson and Shackel, 1998) and synchronous fruit production (Johnson and

175

Weinbaum, 1987; Lyles et al., 2009; Rosenstock et al., 2011). Here, we compare

176

both the synchrony S Y NC and variability CV of the model without noise (detailed

177

in (Satake and Iwasa, 2002a) and equivalent to our model with a constant β x (t)) to

178

that of the model with noise in density-dependent pollen limitation (β x (t)), and the

179

model with noise in accumulated resources/threshold for reproduction (detailed in

180

(Satake and Iwasa, 2002b) and equivalent to our model with constant β x (t) and a

181

noise term added to the second equation).

182

3.1. Synchrony

183

Pollen dispersal distance (D) has a dramatic effect on synchrony, hence mast-

184

ing,in the model. In the absence of noise, S Y NC increases with pollen dispersal 9

185

distance, for a given cost of reproduction k and average density-dependent pollen

186

limitation βavg (Figure 1). That is, as the pollen dispersal distance is increased,

187

masting is observed for larger ranges of k. 2

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

2

k

3

4

5

0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 1

1

1.5 Average β

1.5 Average β

2

0

0.2 1

(a) D = 1 1

Average β

Average β

0.4

0.5

5

0

4

5

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 3

4

1.5

0.6 1

k

3

2

0.8

1.5

2

k

(b) D = 2

2

1

2

0

0.2 1

(c) D = 5

2

k

3

4

5

0

(d) D = 13 (Global pollen coupling)

Figure 1: No noise (β x (t) constant): S Y NC values of model versus k and βavg as pollen dispersal distance D is varied. S Y NC values increase with pollen dispersal distance D and densitydependent pollen limitation βavg , and decrease with increasing cost of reproduction k.

188

At a given set of parameter values (k, βavg ), variability in density-dependent

189

pollen limitation enhances synchrony, hence masting if both the spatial correlation

190

R and variability of the noise cv are large enough, as illustrated in Figure 2 for the

191

case of nearest neighbor coupling (D = 1). For example, when R = 0 increasing

192

cv causes S Y NC to decrease (compare Figures 2(a) & 2(b) to Figure 1(a)). In

193

contrast, when R = 0.5, increasing cv increases the S Y NC value at a given com-

194

bination of k and βavg (compare Figures 2(c) & 2(d) to Figure 1(a)). The same is 10

195

true for the model with noise in accumulated resources/threshold for reproduction

196

(Figure 3). The results for other pollen dispersal distances examined (D = 5 and

197

D = 13/Global) are analogous to those of the nearest neighbor case (unpublished

198

results). That is, both types of noise increase the parameter range of masting when

199

spatial correlation and variability of the noise are both large enough. 2

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

2

k

3

4

5

0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 1

1

1.5 Average β

1.5 Average β

2

0

0.2 1

(a) R = 0, cv = 0.1 1

Average β

Average β

0.4

0.5

4

5

0

5

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 3

4

1.5

0.6 1

k

3

2

0.8

1.5

2

k

(b) R = 0, cv = 0.5

2

1

2

0

0.2 1

(c) R = 0.5, cv = 0.1

2

k

3

4

5

0

(d) R = 0.5, cv = 0.5

Figure 2: Noise in Pollination Efficiency: S Y NC values of model versus cost of reproduction k and average density-dependent pollen limitation βavg for various combinations of correlation R and coefficient of variation cv, under local pollen coupling (D = 1).

200

For the rest of this paper, we focus on a slice of k − β parameter space where

201

βavg = 1 and k varies. We do this to decrease the number of free parameters

202

and choose this particular slice because it seems to include a complete range of

203

possible behavior (see Figure 1). We also fix the spatial correlation for both types

204

of noise at r, R = 0.8 (highly correlated). We will consider both large (σ, cv = 11

2

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

2

k

3

4

5

0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 1

1

1.5 Average β

1.5 Average β

2

0

0.2 1

(a) r = 0, σ = 0.1 1

Average β

Average β

0.4

0.5

4

5

0

5

1 0.8 0.6

1

0.4

0.5

0.2 3

4

1.5

0.6 1

k

3

2

0.8

1.5

2

k

(b) r = 0, σ = 0.5

2

1

2

0

0.2 1

(c) r = 0.5, σ = 0.1

2

k

3

4

5

0

(d) r = 0.5, σ = 0.5

Figure 3: Noise in accumulated resources/threshold for reproduction: S Y NC values of model versus cost of reproduction k and average density-dependent pollen limitation βavg for various combinations of correlation r and coefficient of variation σ, under local pollen coupling (D = 1).

12

205

0.5) and small (σ, cv = 0.1) noise and nearest-neighbor, intermediate, and global

206

pollen coupling distances (D = 1, 5, or D = 13/Global).

207

3.2. Variability

208

For the case with no noise, the coefficient of variation of reproductive output

209

(CV) is greater than one-half for k in [1, 2] under nearest neighbor coupling, and

210

the range of k for which CV is large enough increases as D is increased (Figure

211

4(a)), in parallel with the S Y NC results and confirming the presence of masting

212

under these conditions.

213

The inclusion of noise in density-dependent pollen limitation β x (t) may result

214

in near constant reproduction (CV less than that of the environment), resource

215

matching (variability/CV mirrors that of the environment), or masting (CV at least

216

twice as big as that of the environment), depending upon both the CV of the noise

217

and the extent of pollen coupling D. Reproductive behavior is nearly unchanged

218

from the deterministic (no noise) case for any cost of reproduction k, when both

219

the variability of β x (t) and the extent of spatial coupling are small (D = 1, Figure

220

4(b)). Resource matching is observed for k outside the interval [1,2] whenever

221

β x (t) is noisy enough and pollen coupling is limited to nearest neighbors (D = 1,

222

4(c)). Otherwise, when the spatial extent of pollen coupling is much larger (D = 5

223

or global pollen coupling), masting is observed for large noise in β x (t) (Figure

224

4(c)). In contrast to the case of noise in density-dependent pollen limitation, the

225

noise in accumulated resources/threshold for reproduction is always amplified by

226

the system under the parameter regime studied. This is illustrated in Figures 4(e)

227

and 4(d), where CV of the reproductive output is always larger than that of the

228

noise term in the model. Thus, there is never resource matching because environ-

229

mental variability is always amplified. Similarly, there is always masting if the 13

230

noise is large enough, so that the amplified environmental noise yields a CV of

231

reproductive output greater than one-half.

232

3.3. Types of Masting

233

As discussed in section 1, masting may be divided into three categories: strict,

234

normal bimodal, and normal switching (Kelly, 1994). In determining whether and

235

what type of masting is present, we will consider the long-term behavior of the

236

reproductive output of the model under nearest-neighbor coupling (D = 1). In the

237

absence of noise, masting only occurs when k is between 1 and 2 and it is strict

238

(Figure 5(a)). When large noise in density-dependent pollen limitation β x (t) is

239

included in the model, strict and possibly normal bimodal masting are observed

240

for k values in an interval around k = 1, and normal switching masting is observed

241

for k > 2 (Figure 5(b)). Similarly, when noise in accumulated resources/threshold

242

for reproduction is included in the model normal switching masting is observed

243

for most k values, with the exception of normal bimodal and/or strict masting for

244

a range of parameters near k = 1 and when the noise is small enough (Figures 5(c)

245

and 5(d)). The results for global coupling are analogous.

246

4. Discussion

247

In agreement with previous work (Satake and Iwasa, 2002b), we show that

248

including the biological realism of noise increases the parameter range for which

249

masting may occur. However, our results suggest that the type of environmental

250

noise a plant population experiences will have a profound effect on the dynamics

251

of reproductive output. This highlights the importance of choosing appropriate

252

noise sources in mathematical models and is described in more detail below.

14

1.5 1.2 1 CV

CV

1 0.8 0.6

0.5

0.4 0.2 0 0

1

2

k

3

4

0 0

5

1.2

k

3

4

5

1.2

1

1

0.8

0.8

CV

CV

2

(b) Noisy Pollination, cv = 0.1

(a) No noise

0.6 0.4

0.6 0.4

0.2 0 0

1

0.2 1

2

k

3

4

0 0

5

(c) Noisy Pollination, cv = 0.5

1

2

k

3

4

5

(d) Noisy Resources, σ = 0.1

CV

2

1.5

1 0

1

2

k

3

4

5

(e) Noisy Resources, σ = 0.5 Figure 4: Coefficient of variation of reproductive output (CV) under different types of noise. D = 1 (open circles), D = 5 (closed circles), and Global (stars). Simulations were run for 1,000 years and the coefficient of variation CV of reproductive output was calculated. Note the different scale in (d).

15

1

0.8

0.8

Reproductive Output

Reproductive Output

1

0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

1

2

k

3

4

0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

5

1

2

k

3

4

5

(b) Noisy Pollination, cv = 0.5

(a) No noise

4

Reproductive Output

Reproductive Output

5

3 2 1 0 0

1

2

k

3

4

1.2 1 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 0

5

(c) Noisy Resources, σ = 0.5

1

2

k

3

4

5

(d) Noisy Resources, σ = 0.1

Figure 5: Long term behavior for nearest neighbor coupling (D = 1). Note the different scale in (c). Simulations were run for 200 years and the last 100 years were plotted versus k. Results were similar when the simulations were run for 1,000 years, with the last 100 years shown.

16

253

Plant reproductive variability has been divided into five categories: nearly

254

constant, resource matching, and three types of masting behavior (strict, normal

255

bimodal, and normal switching). Since all plants are subject to environmental

256

variability, our results suggest that (pollen-limited) plant populations that exhibit

257

constant reproductive output, resource matching, and (to a lesser extent) strict

258

masting or normal bimodal masting may respond to noise in density-dependent

259

pollen limitation rather than noise in the threshold for reproduction and/or accu-

260

mulated resources. Those that exhibit the normal switching type of masting may

261

be sensitive to either type of noise. However, density-dependent pollen limitation

262

is likely to be highly variable and discrete because weather events may cause nu-

263

merous phenological responses. For example, partial failure of the pollination pro-

264

cess may be caused by poor male-female flower synchronization (Schauber et al.,

265

2002; Post, 2003) and precipitation during flowering (Knapp et al., 2001; Knops

266

et al., 2007). Thus, noise in density-dependent pollen limitation is a likelier candi-

267

date for the cause of normal switching masting in most pollen-limited plant pop-

268

ulations. Combined with the fact that noise in accumulated resources/threshold

269

for reproduction is largely ineffective for inducing masting in non-pollen-limited

270

plant populations (Satake and Iwasa, 2002b), while noise in pollination efficiency

271

induces masting over a wide parameter range in such plants (Lyles et al., 2009),

272

we suggest that noise in the pollination process is likely to be a proximate mech-

273

anism for variable reproduction in most plants, except for possibly the most vari-

274

able, pollen-limited ones. We hypothesize that the main mechanism by which

275

variability in accumulated resources/threshold for reproduction enhances mast-

276

ing behavior in the models is through adding variability in the pollen limitation

277

term. In contrast, including noise in density-dependent pollen limitation does not

17

278

change the range of y or the range of the pollen limitation term.

279

Our results also show that the presence of switching in the dynamics of a par-

280

ticular plant’s reproduction does not preclude the possibility that the reproductive

281

output of a population of such plants will exhibit resource matching or even near

282

constant reproduction. Moreover, the presence of switching does not confirm that

283

true masting is occurring in the absence of an idea of how variable the environment

284

is. Furthermore, given similar environmental variability, some plant populations

285

may exhibit masting, while others show resource matching or constant reproduc-

286

tion.

287

One may ask why our CV’s of reproductive output for the model with noisy

288

β x (t) are “small” compared to many published values for masting (Kelly, 1994;

289

Kelly et al., 2000; Kelly and Sork, 2002; Kelly et al., 2008). First, researchers

290

may focus on the most variable of species and/or those with zero reproduction

291

between mast years. As discussed in the first paragraph of the results section,

292

for a given amount of variability (standard deviation), the CV approaches infinity

293

as the mean approaches zero and is therefore sensitive to small changes in the

294

mean. Thus, plants with mean reproduction near zero may have CV larger than

295

one while a plant population with similar variability but mean farther from zero

296

has CV less than one. The historical origin of using a “null” CV of 1 for non-

297

masting species probably comes from comparing reproduction to a Poisson dis-

298

tribution (with mean=variance). But it’s not clear that plant reproduction should

299

be Poisson distributed, other than the fact that seeds are count data. Second, some

300

plant populations may be responding to noise in accumulated resources/threshold

301

for reproduction, which can cause higher CV’s in the models. And third, the

302

variability of density-dependent pollen limitation β x (t) may be more discrete than

18

303

represented by normally distributed noise. Although, the climate may vary ac-

304

cording to a normal distribution, its effect on the pollination process can be almost

305

all-or-none.

306

Given that the scale of pollen coupling appears to greatly influence whether or

307

not masting will occur, the persistence of masting behavior may be increasingly

308

threatened by large scale global change processes such as habitat fragmentation

309

and global climate change. Our results suggest that habitat fragmentation, which

310

is occurring at alarming rates in many parts of the world, will decrease pollen

311

exchange and inhibit pollen coupling between disparate individuals and popula-

312

tions. This is consistent with empirical studies that have shown negative effects

313

of pollen limitation on reproductive success in fragmented landscapes (Aguilar

314

et al., 2006). The consequences of global climate change on pollen coupling and

315

masting is less clear because the effect is likely to be species specific. Depend-

316

ing on the precise weather cue a plant population responds to, which can include

317

precipitation and temperatures at various parts of the year (Kelly and Sork, 2002),

318

and its particular values for k and average β x (t) (βavg ), climate change may either

319

favor or prohibit flowering synchrony. For example, species that respond to warm

320

temperatures to initiate bud induction (Kelly et al., 2000, 2008) may produce flow-

321

ers and seed more frequently while many species may suffer reproductive failure

322

due to more regular frost events (Inouye, 2000). Nevertheless the significance of

323

pollen coupling in our results suggests that climate change and habitat fragmenta-

324

tion, together, present considerable challenges to the longevity of pollen mediated

325

masting species.

19

326

5. Acknowledgements

327

Danielle Lyles was supported with an NSF Mathematical Sciences Postdoc-

328

toral Research Fellowship, award number DMS -0703700. Alan Hastings was

329

supported with NSF grants EF-0827460 and 1344187. Todd Rosenstock was sup-

330

ported by a grant from the California Pistachio Research Board and the Cali-

331

fornian Department of Food and Agriculture, Fertilizer Research and Education

332

Program.

333

S. E. Kingsland, Modeling nature: episodes in the history of population ecology,

334

335

336

337

338

339

340

University of Chicago Press, 1995. P. Moran, The statistical analysis of the canadian lynx cycle II: Synchronization and meteorology, Australian Journal of Zoology 1 (1953) 291–298. R. Lande, S. Engen, B.-E. Saether, Stochastic Population Dynamics in Ecology and Conservation, Oxford Series in Ecology and Evolution, 2003. J. H. Connell, W. P. Sousa, On the evidence needed to judge ecological stability or persistence, American Naturalist 121 (1983) 789–824.

341

A. Liebhold, W. D. Koenig, O. N. Bjørnstad, Spatial synchrony in population

342

dynamics, Annual Review of Ecology, Evolution and Systematics 35 (2004)

343

467–490.

344

345

346

347

D. Kelly, The evolutionary ecology of mast seeding, TREE 9 (12) (1994) 465– 470. D. Kelly, V. L. Sork, Mast Seeding in Perennial Plants: Why, How, Where?, Annual Review of Ecology and Systematics 33 (2002) 427–447. 20

348

349

350

351

G. Piovesan, J. M. Adams, Masting behaviour in beech: linking reproduction and climatic variation, Canadian Journal of Botany 79 (2001) 1039–1047. M. Rees, D. Kelly, O. N. Bjørnstad, Snow Tussocks, Chaos, and the Evolution of Mast Seeding, The American Naturalist 160 (1) (2002) 44–59.

352

E. M. Schauber, D. Kelly, P. Turchin, C. Simon, W. G. Lee, R. B. Allen, I. J.

353

Payton, P. R. Wilson, P. E. Cowan, R. E. Brockie, Masting by Eighteen New

354

Zealand Plant Species: The Role of Temperature as a Synchronizing Cue, Ecol-

355

ogy 83 (5) (2002) 1214–1225.

356

D. Kelly, A. L. Harrison, W. G. Lee, I. J. Payton, P. R. Wilson, E. M. Schauber,

357

Predator Satiation and Extreme Mast Seeding in 11 Species of Chionochloa

358

(Poaceae), Oikos 90 (3) (2000) 477–488.

359

D. Kelly, M. H. Turnbull, R. P. Pharis, M. S. Sarfati, Mast seeding, predator sati-

360

ation, and temperature cues in Chionochloa (Poaceae), Population Ecology 50

361

(2008) 343–355.

362

V. Sel˙as, Seed production of a masting dwarf shrub, Vaccinium myrtillus, in re-

363

lation to previous reproduction and weather, Canadian Journal of Botany 78

364

(2000) 423–429.

365

H. Kon, T. Noda, K. Terazawa, H. Koyama, M. Yasaka, Proximate factors causing

366

mast seeding in Fagus crenata: the effects of resource level and weather cues,

367

Canadian Journal of Botany 83 (2005) 1402–1409.

368

369

H. Kon, T. Noda, Experimental investigation on weather cues for mast seeding of fagus crenata, Ecological Research 22 (2007) 802–806. 21

370

J. M. Lamontagne, S. Boutin, Local-scale synchrony and variability in mast seed

371

production patterns of picea glauca, Journal of Ecology 95 (5) (2007) 991–

372

1000.

373

374

Y. Isagi, K. Sugimura, A. Sumida, H. Ito, How Does Masting Happen and Synchronize?, Journal of Theoretical Biology 187 (1997) 231–239.

375

A. Satake, Y. Iwasa, Pollen Coupling of Forest Trees: Forming Synchronized and

376

Periodic Reproduction out of Chaos, Journal of Theoretical Biology 203 (2000)

377

63–84.

378

379

A. Satake, Y. Iwasa, Spatially Limited Pollen Exchange and a Long-Range Synchronization of Trees, Ecology 83 (4) (2002a) 993–1005.

380

A. Satake, Y. Iwasa, The synchronized and intermittent reproduction of forest

381

trees is mediated by the Moran effect, only in association with pollen coupling,

382

Journal of Ecology 90 (2002b) 830–838.

383

D. Lyles, T. S. Rosenstock, A. Hastings, P. H. Brown, The role of large envi-

384

ronmental noise in masting: General model and example from pistachio trees,

385

Journal of Theoretical Biology 259 (4) (2009) 701–713.

386

387

388

389

390

J. R. Obeso, The costs of reproduction in plants, New Phytologist 155 (2002) 321–348. M. Burd, Bateman’s Principle and Plant Reproduction: The Role of Pollen Limitation in Fruit and Seed Set, The Botanical Review 60 (1) (1994) 83–139. R. Aguilar, L. Ashworth, L. Galetto, M. A. Alzen, Plant reproductive succepti-

22

391

bility to habitat fragmentation: review and synthesis through a meta-analysis,

392

Ecology Letters 9 (2006) 968–980.

393

394

E. Post, Large-scale climate synchronizes the timing of flowering by multiple species, Ecology 84 (2) (2003) 277–281.

395

E. E. Knapp, M. A. Goedde, K. J. Rice, Pollen-limited reproduction in blue oak:

396

implications for wind pollination in fragmented populations, Oecologia 128

397

(2001) 48–55.

398

J. M. Knops, W. D. Koenig, W. J. Carmen, Negative correlation does not imply a

399

tradeoff between growth and reproduction in California Oaks, PNAS 104 (43)

400

(2007) 16982–16985.

401

402

403

404

405

406

J. Matthews, The Influence of Weather on the Frequency of Beech Mast Years in England, Forestry (1955) 107–116. D. W. Inouye, The ecological and evolutionary significance of frost in the context of climate change, Ecology Letters 3 (2000) 457–463. W. D. Koenig, M. V. Ashley, Is pollen limited? The answer is blowin in the wind, TRENDS in Ecology and Evolution 18 (4) (2003) 157–159.

407

Y. Iwasa, A. Satake, Mechanisms inducing spatially extended synchrony in mast

408

seeding: The role of pollen coupling and environmental fluctuation, Ecological

409

Research 19 (2004) 13–20.

410

M. T. Stevenson, K. A. Shackel, Alternate Bearing in Pistachio as a Masting Phe-

411

nomenon: Construction Cost of Reproduction versus Vegetative Growth and

23

412

Storage, Journal of the American Society for Horticultural Science 123 (6)

413

(1998) 1069–1075.

414

R. S. Johnson, S. A. Weinbaum, Variation in tree size, cropping efficiency, and al-

415

ternate bearing among kerman pistachio trees, Journal of the American Society

416

for Horticultural Science 112 (6) (1987) 942–945.

417

418

419

420

421

422

T. S. Rosenstock, A. Hastings, W. D. Koenig, D. J. Lyles, P. H. Brown, Testing Moran’s Theorem in an Agroecosystem, Oikos 120 (9) (2011) 1434–1440. W. E. Kunin, Sex and the Single Mustard: Population Density and Pollinator Behavior Effects on Seed-Set, Ecology 74 (7) (1993) 2145–2160. E. E. Crone, Responses of Social and Solitary Bees to Pulsed Floral Resources, The American Naturalist 182 (4) (2013) 465–473.

24

Plant reproduction and environmental noise: how do plants do it?

Plant populations exhibit a wide continuum of reproductive behavior, ranging from nearly constant reproductive output on one end to the extreme of mas...
740KB Sizes 0 Downloads 10 Views