Journal of Developmental Origins of Health and Disease (2012), 3(3), 190–197. & Cambridge University Press and the International Society for Developmental Origins of Health and Disease 2012 doi:10.1017/S2040174412000141

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Placental measurements associated with intelligence quotient at age 7 years D. P. Misra1*, C. M. Salafia2,3, A. K. Charles4 and R. K. Miller5 1

Department of Family Medicine, Wayne State University, Detroit, MI, USA Placental Analytics, LLC, Larchmont, NY, USA 3 Institute for Basic Research, Staten Island, NY, USA 4 Department of Pathology, Princess Margaret Hospital, Perth, WA, Australia 5 Department of Obstetrics and Gynecology, University of Rochester, Rochester NY, USA 2

We hypothesized that placental villous branching that is measured by disk chorionic plate expansion and disk thickness is correlated with factors also involved in regulation of branching growth of other fetal viscera (e.g. lung, kidney) including neuronal dendrites, and thus may be associated with variation in childhood intelligence quotient (IQ). IQ at age 7 years was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children. Placental measures [placental weight (g), thickness (mm), chorionic plate surface diameters (cm), area (cm2), shape, and cord length and cord eccentricity] were independent variables in regression analyses of age 7-year IQ in 12,926 singleton term live born infants with complete placental data. Analyses were stratified on gender with adjustment for socioeconomic status, race, parity, gestational age, exact age at testing and centered parental ages. After adjustment for covariates, placental measurements were independently associated with IQ at age 7 years but results varied by gender. Chorionic plate diameters were only associated with higher IQ in girls. Placental thickness was positively associated with higher IQ for boys and girls. We have previously shown that placental measures affect age 7-year body mass index and diastolic blood pressure. Here we demonstrate that specific measures, placental chorionic plate diameters in girls and disk thickness, independent of gender, are correlated with age 7-year IQ. Further exploration of the possible interaction of these factors on the placental villous arborization reflected by the chorionic plate expansion and placental thickness that correlate with age 7-year IQ, as well as other age 7 somatic features as previously addressed, is indicated. Received 16 June 2011; Revised 15 February 2012; Accepted 27 February 2012; First published online 20 March 2012 Key words: gender, intelligence, placenta

Introduction More than a century of research has sought to identify the predictors of childhood cognitive function, with a growing focus on the in utero environment, thus paralleling research on predictors of childhood and adult health. Numerous precedents exist; within the framework of research on fetal origins of chronic disease, there is now considerable evidence that should direct attention upstream to those factors at play at the time a child is born, namely to the intrauterine environment. The placenta has been suggested to be a window into the intrauterine environment through which the accrued consequences of exposures, genetic susceptibilities and their interactions can be inferred from the earliest stages of pregnancy.1–6 The data relating the intrauterine period to the origins to at least some types of altered brain function is also robust (e.g.7,8). Recent studies have focused on the possible role of paternal age on offspring characteristics including childhood intelligence quotient (IQ).9 However, while placental growth (as represented by placental weight and its derivatives including its *Address for correspondence: Dr D. P. Misra, Department of Family Medicine, Wayne State University, 3939 Woodward Avenue, Room 318, Detroit, MI, USA 48170. (Email [email protected])

shape) has been linked to child and adult health,10–14 no prior published studies have reported associations with IQ in childhood or adulthood. We sought to extend our work on the link between placental growth as reflected in placental shape and childhood and adult health14–19 to the outcome of childhood IQ. We and others assert that the delivered placenta is a representation, or a summary, of the exposures and experiences of the in utero environment of the child. This would suggest that there may be an association between measures of placenta and childhood IQ as other evidence indicates the potential importance of prenatal factors.20–27 To date, prior studies of the relationship between the placenta and child and adult outcomes have rarely utilized measures other than simply placental weight and the placental weight–birth weight ratio (or its inverse, the fetoplacental weight ratio). Placental weight is but a summary of many dimensions of placental villous growth, including the placental chorionic surface area, and the thickness of the placental chorionic disk. The placental chorionic surface area reflects the laterally expanding growth of the placenta (measured by disk shape, the distance from the cord insertion site to the nearest disk margin and by chorionic disk diameters). This aspect of placental growth serves two functions. First, the umbilical–chorionic vessels run in the chorionic plate. These large caliber vessels bear the burden of transfer of large volumes

Placental measurements associated with IQ at age 7 years of fetal blood to and from the villus capillary bed. Thus, disk shape, distance from cord insertion to margin and large/small chorionic disk diameters are each measures that capture dimensions of this high capacitance/low resistance flow system. Second, the chorionic disk area measures the area of the uterine lining covered by the placenta28 and in effect, how many maternal spiral arteries are potential suppliers of the placenta. Placental chorionic disk thickness reflects the arborization of the fetal stem villi with their arterioles, which terminate in terminal villi, the locus of the villous and vascular nutrient exchange surface.29 However, the fetal stem arterioles are principal sites of placental vascular resistance,30,31 thus they contribute to total fetal peripheral resistance and fetal heart work.32 In contrast to the more global measures of placental weight and the ratio to birth weight, analyses of these more nuanced measures could shed light on the mechanisms linking placental growth to later cognitive outcomes, as they have for somatic outcomes.14 Using data from the U.S. Collaborative Perinatal Project (CPP), we sought to determine how placental growth measures relate to child IQ at age 7 years. We considered a number of standard placental measures of growth and development as potential predictors of childhood IQ. On the basis of recent studies implicating parental age and sex differential effects therein, we also considered the role of mother’s and father’s age and whether the placental measures’ effects differed for boys and girls. Methods Subjects were a subset of the National CPP. Details of the study have been described elsewhere.9,10 Briefly, from 1959 to 1965, women who attended prenatal care at 12 hospitals were invited to participate in the observational, prospective study. At entry, detailed demographic, socioeconomic and behavioral information was collected by in-person interview. A medical history, physical examination and blood sample were also obtained. In subsequent prenatal visits, women were repeatedly interviewed and physical findings were recorded. During labor and delivery, placental gross morphology was examined and samples were collected for histologic examination. The children were followed up to 7 years of age. In the full CPP cohort, there were 55,740 singleton pregnancies. The full CPP data set includes more than one pregnancy to the same mother over the 7 years of recruitment, necessitating selection of a subset or use of methods to account for the lack of independence of the observations. The sample was therefore restricted to only or first singleton live births within a family. Among 41,341 eligible singleton births, 36,017 contributed placenta data (, 87%). The analytic sample was restricted to those with complete data on the necessary placental gross measures (see next paragraph), placental weight, birth weight and known gestational age >34 weeks and ,43 completed weeks (n 5 24,152, 67%). The lowest 0.5% of placental variables was also excluded, as biologically implausible, leaving a sample of 24,061 subjects with placental and pregnancy

191

data.18,19 We then excluded observations with missing data on key variables for analyses focused on in this manuscript: maternal and/or paternal age; race categorized as AfricanAmerican or white; socioeconomic status (SES); age 7-year IQ test results; exact age at the 7-year IQ testing. This sample composition (n 5 12,883) is consistent with prior analyses of age 7-year IQ in this data set by other investigators,33 albeit smaller due to the need for complete placental data (20,827 v. 12,883) and potentially a result of sampling differences (choosing a random birth rather than the first or only birth). Comparing births in the CPP with and without placental data suggests that there is no major difference in the samples (see18,19,34). Placental variables Placental gross measures were collected according to a standard protocol;35 the original codings and recodings used for this analysis follow: (1) Placental weight. Placental weight was measured in decagrams to the nearest 10 g; the unit of analysis was grams. (2, 3) Placental (chorionic plate) surface diameters (large, small). Diameters (large and small) of the chorionic plate surface were recorded in centimeters, and analyzed as coded. (4) Placenta (chorionic plate) surface area (cm2). This is a mathematical product of the larger and smaller chorionic plate surface diameters. (5) Placental (chorionic disk) thickness. Placental chorionic disk thickness at the center of the chorionic plate was measured by piercing the disk with a knitting needle on which millimeter marks were inscribed, and analyzed in units of 0.1 cm. (6) Placental (chorionic plate) shape. Chorionic plate shape coding was based on the gross examination of the delivered placenta. Surface shapes included round-to-oval, and a variety of atypical shapes (e.g. bipartite, tripartite, succenturiate, membranous, crescent or ‘irregular’). Only 926 (3.8%) were labeled as one of the six categories of shape other than roundto-oval. For this analysis, shape was recoded as a binary variable with ‘round-to-oval’ as ‘0’ and ‘other than round-to-oval’ as ‘1’. (7) Umbilical cord length. Umbilical cord length was measured in the Labor and Delivery Room, and recorded in centimeters. (8) Umbilical cord eccentricity. Distance from the umbilical cord insertion to the closest edge of the placental chorionic plate was recorded to the nearest centimeters. Umbilical cord insertion was also coded as membranous (velamentous), marginal or normal (inserted onto the chorionic plate surface). We combined these two variables into a single umbilical cord insertion distance measure, by recoding velamentous cord insertions as a negative value, cords inserted at the placental margin as ‘0’ and progressively more central cords as ‘1’ to ‘9’ (overall scale range: 13–13; further details available from the authors). Parental characteristics Parental characteristics were recorded at enrollment. Paternal and maternal ages were measured (at enrollment) in years and

192

D. P. Misra et al.

months with transformation to centered variables to minimize problems with collinearity.36 Parity counted all delivered offspring and did not include miscarriages/early pregnancy losses; parity was recoded into a binary variable (0 5 nulliparous, 1 5 parous). SES index was a combined score for education, occupation and family income as scaled by the US Bureau of the Census.37 Mother’s race was coded as a binary variable denoting African-American as ‘1’ and all others as ‘0’; original data coded race as Caucasian, African-American, and ‘other’, most of whom were Puerto Ricans (9.2%). IQ at age 7 years was assessed using the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children (WISC), widely regarded as a valid and reliable measure of intelligence in children.38 Preliminary analyses indicated gender-modified effects of key variables consisted with prior work by our team.15 Therefore, analyses were stratified on gender with covariates of SES, African-American race, parity, gestational age and exact age at WISC (IQ) testing. Placental weight (g), chorionic plate surface diameters (cm) and chorionic plate surface area (cm2), chorionic disk thickness (mm), shape, umbilical cord length (cm) and umbilical cord eccentricity were independent variables in linear regression analyses (SPSS-PC) of age 7-year IQ adjusted for the exact age at testing in 12,883 singleton term liveborns with complete placental data. Prior work further suggested the potential for nonlinearities in placental variable effects15 so we considered linear regression with categorical predictors.

Because of instability of models, we explored these effects only for variables that had similar effects for boys and girls so that data could be combined. Results In Table 1 we described the means and distribution of a number of birth and placental characteristics measured in this study. The 12,883 singleton births were approximately evenly distributed by sex with 49% female. The racial distribution was less evenly distributed with slightly more than one-third (37.2%) reported as African-American. All models were adjusted for exact age at the 7-year-old IQ testing. We first examined the effects of each placental measure alone. All of the covariates we considered for inclusion have been reported by us or others to be related to both IQ and placental factors. Furthermore, there is biologic plausibility for some of these associations (e.g. nulliparity, gestational age). In addition, we conducted analyses to test the association between each of the covariates and each placental factor. Then we considered effects of placental measures in a single model together after taking account of parental, perinatal and child factors as these factors were likely to confound effects of placental factors. We compared the estimate of the regression coefficient and the model fit with the covariate both included and excluded from our models of placental variables and IQ. Table 2 provides the results for the simple models of the

Table 1. Descriptive characteristics of the study sample Males (n 5 6625)

Child’s IQ at age 7 years*** Maternal age Paternal age* Race* African-American White Gestational age (weeks)*** Nulliparous (no prior live birth) Placental weight (g)** Socioeconomic status (needs/income ratio)** Range 1–5 Child’s age at testing (weeks) Placenta largest dimension (cm)* Placenta smallest dimension (cm)* Placental area (cm2)** Placental thickness (mm) Placental shape not round/oval* Relative cord Eccentricitya**

Mean (S.D.)/percent (n)

Range

Mean (S.D.)/percent (n)

98.2 6 17.2 24.8 6 6.0 28.1 6 7.2

17–181 14–46 14–66

101.1 6 17.6 24.8 6 6.0 28.4 6 7.3

6–169 14–47 14–66

34–43

38.2% (2390) 61.8% (3868) 39.7 6 1.9 34.2% (n 5 2140) 437 6 92

34–43

36.2% (2400) 63.8% (4225) 39.5 6 1.9 35.6% (n 5 2361) 443 6 92 3.21 6 1.1 384.5 6 34.7 19.2 6 2.1 16.5 6 1.9 250 6 49 2.2 6 0.5 4.0% (268) 0.31 6 0.14

IQ, intelligence quotient. Cord location relative to disk edge normalized to radius of disk. *P , 0.05; **P , 0.01; ***P , 0.001.

a

Females (n 5 6258)

175–850 1–5 302–410 12–30 7–24 92–484 0.6–4.0 21.1, 0.8

3.20 6 1.1 383.5 6 35.4 19.0 6 2.1 16.4 6 1.9 247 6 49 2.2 6 0.5 4.7% (297) 0.30 6 0.14

Range

190–990 1–5 303–410 13–30 7–24 88–509 0.8–4.3 20.8–0.7

Table 2. Placental predictors of WISC Full Scale IQ (points) at age 7 years Males Unadjusted linear regression b (95% CI)

0.01 0.50 20.46 0.00 0.35 3.86 212.19 0.07

(20.02, 0.03) (20.64, 1.64) (21.73, 0.81) (20.05, 0.05) (20.18, 0.87) (20.35, 16.08) (229.22, 4.84) (0.04, 0.10)***

0.26 (20.14, 0.66) 0.21 (20.13, 0.54) 213.06 (218.08, 28.05)*** 0.66 (20.59, 1.90) 23.38 (28.41, 1.64) 5.80 (3.58, 8.02)***

WISC, Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children; IQ, intelligence quotient. All models control for child’s exact chronological age at age 7-year testing. a Adjusted models include all covariates listed in this table. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.

Adjusted linear regressiona b (95% CI)

20.007 1.19 0.87 20.06 0.30 1.82 1.13 0.02

(20.013, 20.002)* (20.04, 2.40) (20.63, 2.40) (20.15, 0.04) (0.21, 0.40)*** (20.09, 3.73) (21.5, 3.75) (0, 0.05)*

0.20 (0.10, 0.31)*** 20.11 (20.20, 0.03)** 26.16 (27.00, 25.31)*** 0.28 (0.09, 0.47)** 20.47 (21.37, 0.42) 5.33 (4.95, 5.70)***

Unadjusted linear regression b (95% CI)

0.02 1.26 1.48 0.07 0.39 22.88 21.96 0.10

(0.000, 0.05) (0.19, 2.34) (0.25, 2.70) (0.02, 0.11) (20.11, 0.89) (213.62, 7.85) (217.92, 14.00) (0.07, 0.13)***

Adjusted linear regressiona b (95% CI)

0.00 1.52 1.56 20.09 0.24 20.59 0.59 0.03

(20.01, 0.01) (0.23, 2.81)* (0.03, 3.09)* (20.19, 0.01) (0.14, 0.34)*** (22.42, 1.24) (22.09, 3.28) (0.00, 0.06)*

0.52 (0.14, 0.90)** 0.15 (20.17, 0.46) 215.51 (220.21, 210.81)

0.41 (0.30, 0.52)** 20.16 (0.25, 0.08)** 26.57 (27.44, 25.71)***

1.30 (0.12, 2.47)*** 0.99 (23.81, 5.79)

0.38 (0.18, 0.58)*** 21.42 (22.35, 20.50)**

4.10 (2.02, 6.17)***

5.74 (5.37, 6.12)***

Placental measurements associated with IQ at age 7 years

Placental Placental weight (g) Larger chorionic plate diameter (cm) Smaller chorionic plate diameter (cm) Chorionic plate area (cm2) Placental (chorionic plate) thickness (mm) Shape Cord eccentricity Umbilical cord length Parental Maternal age (centered, years) Paternal age (centered, years) African-American (maternal) Perinatal Gestational age (days) Parous (>1 live birth) Child Socioeconomic status (income/needs ratio)

Females

193

194

D. P. Misra et al.

independent variables (placental, parental, perinatal and child) and IQ, as well as the results for the full models (see next paragraph for description of adjusted full models). Race and SES were associated with IQ in both genders. In the models considering each placental measure alone, we found that, in girls only, the greater the larger and smaller placental chorionic plate surface diameters, and the chorionic plate surface area (essentially an interaction term for the larger and smaller diameters), the greater the IQ. Maternal age and gestational age were also each positively associated with increased IQ in girls but not boys. Umbilical cord length was also positively associated with IQ in both genders. After adjustment for parental, perinatal and child covariates (see Table 2), several of the placental measurements independently associated with IQ at age 7 years and again, results varied by gender. The tests for interactions between gender and placental factors in these cases (placental chorionic plate surface diameters, umbilical cord length) were statistically significant (P , 0.05).

There was a positive association between placental weight and IQ but only in boys. However, the size of coefficient was very small.

at age 7 years. These findings build on our previous work in this field. First, it should not be unexpected that placental shape measures that are reflections of the cumulative growth of the placenta across gestation affects neurodevelopment given the evidence that the intrauterine environment is significantly correlated with such outcome.7,8,39 The placenta is the fetus’ sole source of oxygen and nutrients and as such would be reasonably expected to influence newborn outcome directly. We have recently shown an allometric scaling relation between the placental weight and the birth weight in the CPP cohort with an exponent approximately equal to 0.75, as predicted by Kleiber’s Law. This implies that placental weight is a justifiable proxy for fetal metabolic rate when other measures of fetal metabolic rate are not available.40 However, placental growth may also mark exposures that alter fetal neurodevelopment, and thus be more indirect measures of the intrauterine forces and factors that modulate fetal neurodevelopment. In addition, placental shape measures may provide insights into timing of gestational stressors sufficient to deform placental growth and potentially affect the fetus.41 Within the CPP cohort, our group has produced a number of important findings of independent effects of placental growth as reflected by measures of the chorionic disk surface and chorionic disk thickness on neonatal and childhood outcomes, as well as gender specificity of such effects. These findings include:

Placenta (chorionic plate) surface diameters

1. Placental shape measures including those of the chorionic

Placental weight

Both the smaller and the larger chorionic plate surface diameters were positively associated with IQ only in girls. Placental (chorionic disk) thickness There was a positive association between placental chorionic disk thickness and IQ for both boys and girls with a similar magnitude of effect. Umbilical cord length There was a positive association with IQ for girls only, with approximately one-third the magnitude of effect of the unadjusted analysis. The R2 (approximate variance explained) by the covariates as a group was , 0.218 for males and 0.257 for females. The addition of the placental variables all together incrementally increased the R2 to 0.223 in males and 0.263 in females. Prior work suggested the potential for nonlinearities in placental effects so additional analyses were performed to examine a range of cutpoints of the predictor as described in the section ‘Methods’. However, due to instability in these models, we have not presented these results. Discussion Our analysis finds that placental measures including measures of the chorionic disk shape, are related to cognitive outcome

plate surface and chorionic disk thickness, have independent significant effects on fetal growth;18 that is, placental weight does not capture all the subtlety of the information about gestational ‘adequacy’ that is encoded in placental shape. 2. Different placental disk proportions yield different mean birth weights, with ranges of fetoplacental weight ratios from 8.46 to 6.33. Taking the mean weight of a wellproportioned placenta (mid quartiles for chorionic plate area and thickness, 435.9 g), these different fetoplacental weight ratios would yield expected birth weights of from 3688 g (actual observed mean birth weight 2823 g) to 2759 g (observed mean birth weight 3620.5 g).19 The impact of changing placental disk proportions, independent of any other factor, is large. 3. Female and male fetuses do not have the same relationships of their birth weights to placental shape measures.15 Female fetuses appear to modulate their growth more closely in step with changing placental disk proportions; male fetuses appear to grow without responding to changing placental shape measures, where these changes may reflect shifts in the intrauterine environment that require adaptation. 4. How large or small a newborn is relative to his/her placental shape measures (as calculated in the observed to expected birth weight ratio) is associated with significant and independent effects on body mass index and diastolic blood pressure.14

Placental measurements associated with IQ at age 7 years In the current study, we extend these observations to IQ, a measure that has been superseded by other measures of cognitive function but which does capture aspects of function that have proved informative in the understanding of cognition in the past.42 We continue to demonstrate both gender-specific and nonlinear effects of placental measures on this measure of neurodevelopmental outcome. To our knowledge, there are no published reports assessing the influence of placental factors on IQ or other measures of cognitive function. We find the association of IQ measures of placental growth and especially placental thickness to be intriguing because the placenta is an ideal site to search for developmental alterations in angiogenesis that may mirror altered neuronal structures that depend on similar regulatory systems.43–45 Our current working hypothesis is that structures that develop by branching have conserved morphogenetic mechanisms.46,47 Placental disk thickness is a direct measure of placental villous and vascular branching growth, which involves branching angiogenesis until the early third trimester.29 Since villous growth is driven by vascular growth,48,49 disk thickness is an indirect measure of placental angiogenesis.50 Placental angiogenesis depends on the same factors that regulate angiogenesis elsewhere in the body.51 These factors have recently been shown to be deeply involved in neuronal development and maintenance. Even vascular endothelial growth factor, once thought to be exclusively involved in angiogenesis have recently been identified as a neurotrophic factor.51 Conversely, factors identified initially for their neurotrophic properties are now recognized as important angiogenic mediators.52 Crosstalk between angiogenic and neurotrophic factors emphasizes the extent to which vascular and neuronal developmental programs are integrated.53–64 The placenta, the anatomical barrier between mother and fetus, is a complex 3D vascular tree covered by a thin veneer of supporting tissue. Forming the boundary between maternal and fetal beings, the placenta is the obvious ‘first responder’ to factors delivered to the conceptus via maternal perfusion,65 including environmental toxicants (e.g. cadmium66), decidually produced trophic factors such as interleukin-10 (e.g.67), as well as factors that impact on cell cycle regulation.68 Our findings that there is a direct and independent relationship between placental chorionic disk thickness, a simple albeit crude (see next paragraph) measure of placental branching vascular growth and IQ, a measure of aspects of neuronal/cognitive function, are yet another piece of evidence that may provide a link between control of vascular and neuronal development. We cannot exclude, as an alternative hypothesis, that the correlation of reduced chorionic disk thickness and age 7-year IQ reflects instead effects of reductions in transplacental transport of nutrients and factors key to neurodevelopment. However, the reductions in birth weight we have observed are small, on the level of tens of grams.18 We think it unlikely that reduced chorionic disk thickness is correlated with age 7-year IQ on the basis of altered placental nutrient transfer function alone.

195

Limitations of this study are that the standard measures of placental shape are imprecise and more poorly measure the most complicated placental shapes, which our data suggest indicate placentas that have been subject to gestational stress. Irregularly shaped placentas with variable disk thickness may have ‘struggled’ more to maintain fetal well-being; a single pair of diameters and a single disk thickness may not capture the complexity of their shape. More precise placental shape measures, as delineated in our most recent work69 exist. As such, we expect that future studies that utilize more precise placental measures will not only confirm the above observations, but illuminate subtleties in the placental shape–IQ relationship that cannot be assessed using the current standard, but crude, measures. There is also an increased potential for false positive findings given the number of placental variables examined. Conclusion IQ at age 7 years was associated with placental factors with apparent differences by gender. Several placental measures, including the more nuanced measure of placental thickness, are also correlated with age 7-year IQ independent of parental, child and perinatal factors, but the findings vary by gender. Further exploration of the possible interaction of these factors on the arborization, reflected by placental chorionic disk thickness, that correlates with age 7-year IQ is indicated. References 1. Hemachandra AH, Klebanoff MA, Duggan AK, Hardy JB, Furth SL. The association between intrauterine growth restriction in the full-term infant and high blood pressure at age 7 years: results from the Collaborative Perinatal Project. Int J Epidemiol. 2006; 35, 871–877. 2. Thame M, Osmond C, Wilks RJ, et al. Blood pressure is related to placental volume and birth weight. Hypertension. 2000; 35, 662–667. 3. Risnes KR, Romundstad PR, Nilsen TIL, Eskild A, Vatten LJ. Placental weight relative to birth weight and long-term cardiovascular mortality: findings from a cohort of 31,307 men and women. Am J Epidemiol. 2009; 170, 622–631. 4. Gatford KL, Simmons RA, De Blasio MJ, Robinson JS, Owens JA. Review: Placental programming of postnatal diabetes and impaired insulin action after IUGR. Placenta. 2010; 31(Suppl.), S60–S65. 5. Gheorghe CP, Goyal R, Mittal A, Longo LD. Gene expression in the placenta: maternal stress and epigenetic responses. Int J Dev Biol. 2010; 54, 507–523. 6. Thompson JA, Regnault TRH. In utero origins of adult insulin resistance and vascular dysfunction. Semin Reprod Med. 2011; 29, 211–224. 7. Rees S, Inder T. Fetal and neonatal origins of altered brain development. Early Hum Dev. 2005; 81, 753–761. 8. Coe CL, Lubach GR. Prenatal origins of individual variation in behavior and immunity. Neurosci Biobehav Rev. 2005; 29, 39–49.

196

D. P. Misra et al.

9. Liu Y, Zhi M, Li X. Parental age and characteristics of the offspring. Ageing Res Rev. 2011; 10, 115–123. 10. Campbell DM, Hall MH, Barker DJ, et al. Diet in pregnancy and the offspring’s blood pressure 40 years later. Br J Obstet Gynaecol. 1996; 103, 273–280. 11. Barker DJ, Bull AR, Osmond C, Simmonds SJ. Fetal and placental size and risk of hypertension in adult life. BMJ. 1990; 301, 259–262. 12. Eriksson J, Forsen T, Tuomilehto J, Osmond C, Barker D. Fetal and childhood growth and hypertension in adult life. Hypertension. 2000; 36, 790–794. 13. Whincup P, Cook D, Papacosta O, Walker M. Birth weight and blood pressure: cross sectional and longitudinal relations in childhood. BMJ. 1995; 311, 773–776. 14. Misra D, CM S, Chadha K, Charles A, Miller RK. Birthweights smaller or larger than the placenta predict BMI and blood pressure at age 7 years. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2010; 1, 123–130. 15. Misra DP, Salafia CM, Miller RK, Charles AK. Non-linear and gender-specific relationships among placental growth measures and the fetoplacental weight ratio. Placenta. 2009; 30, 1052–1057. 16. Salafia CM, Maas E, Thorp JM, et al. Measures of placental growth in relation to birth weight and gestational age. Am J Epidemiol. 2005; 162, 991–998. 17. Salafia CM, Misra DP, Yampolsky M, Charles AK, Miller RK. Allometric metabolic scaling and fetal and placental weight. Placenta. 2009; 30, 355–360. 18. Salafia CM, Zhang J, Charles AK, et al. Placental characteristics and birthweight. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2008; 22, 229–239. 19. Salafia CM, Zhang J, Miller RK, et al. Placental growth patterns affect birth weight for given placental weight. Birth Defects Res A Clin Mol Teratol. 2007; 79, 281–288. 20. Camp BW, Broman SH, Nichols PL, Leff M. Maternal and neonatal risk factors for mental retardation: defining the ‘at-risk’ child. Early Hum Dev. 1998; 50, 159–173. 21. Ellman LM, Yolken RH, Buka SL, Torrey EF, Cannon TD. Cognitive functioning prior to the onset of psychosis: the role of fetal exposure to serologically determined influenza infection. Biol Psychiatry. 2009; 65, 1040–1047. 22. Gray KA, Klebanoff MA, Brock JW, et al. In utero exposure to background levels of polychlorinated biphenyls and cognitive functioning among school-age children. Am J Epidemiol. 2005; 162, 17–26. 23. Holden KR, Mellits ED, Freeman JM. Neonatal seizures. I. Correlation of prenatal and perinatal events with outcomes. Pediatrics. 1982; 70, 165–176. 24. Klebanoff MA, Berendes HW. Aspirin exposure during the first 20 weeks of gestation and IQ at four years of age. Teratology. 1988; 37, 249–255. 25. LeWinn KZ, Stroud LR, Molnar BE, et al. Elevated maternal cortisol levels during pregnancy are associated with reduced childhood IQ. Int J Epidemiol. 2009; 38, 1700–1710. 26. Strauss RS, Dietz WH. Growth and development of term children born with low birth weight: effects of genetic and environmental factors. J Pediatr. 1998; 133, 67–72. 27. Broman S. Prenatal risk factors for mental retardation in young children. Public Health Rep. 1987; 102(Suppl. 4), 55–57.

28. Craven CM, Zhao L, Ward K. Lateral placental growth occurs by trophoblast cell invasion of decidual veins. Placenta. 2000; 21, 160–169. 29. Benirschke K, Kaufmann P, Baergen R. Architecture of normal villous trees. In The Human Placenta, 5th edn (eds. Benirschke K, Kaufman P), 2006; pp. 121–141. Springer: New York. 30. Sagol S, Sagol O, Ozdemir N. Stereological quantification of placental villus vascularization and its relation to umbilical artery Doppler flow in intrauterine growth restriction. Prenat Diagn. 2002; 22, 398–403. 31. Mitra SC, Seshan SV, Riachi LE. Placental vessel morphometry in growth retardation and increased resistance of the umbilical artery Doppler flow. J Matern Fetal Med. 2000; 9, 282–286. 32. Sebire NJ, Talbert D. ‘Cor placentale’: placental intervillus/ intravillus blood flow mismatch is the pathophysiological mechanism in severe intrauterine growth restriction due to uteroplacental disease. Med Hypotheses. 2001; 57, 354–357. 33. Saha S, Barnett AG, Foldi C, et al. Advanced paternal age is associated with impaired neurocognitive outcomes during infancy and childhood. PLoS Med. 2009; 6, e40. 34. Baptiste-Roberts K, Salafia CM, Nicholson WK, et al. Gross placental measures and childhood growth. J Matern Fetal Neona. 2009; 22, 13–23. 35. Benirschke K. Examination of the Placenta; Collaborative Study on Cerebral Palsy, Mental Retardation and Other Neurological and Sensory Disorders of Infancy and Childhood, 1961: National Institute of Neurologic Disease and Blindness, US Department of Health, Education and Welfare. Washington, DC. 36. Wood F. Comment: effect of centering on collinearity and interpretation of the constant. Am Stat. 1984; 38, 88–90. 37. Myrianthopoulos N, French K. An application of the U.S. Bureau of the Census socioeconomic index to a large, diversified patient population. Soc Sci Med. 1968; 2, 283–299. 38. Wechsler. Manual for the Wechsler Intelligence Scale for Children1949. The Psychological Corporation: New York. 39. Clark EAS, Mele L, Wapner RJ, et al. Association of fetal inflammation and coagulation pathway gene polymorphisms with neurodevelopmental delay at age 2 years. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2010; 203, 83.e1–83.e10. 40. Salafia CM, Misra DP, Yampolsky M, Charles AK, Miller RK. Allometric metabolic scaling and fetal and placental weight. Placenta. 2009; 30, 355–360. 41. Salafia CM, Yampolsky M, Shlakhter A, Mandel DH, Schwartz N. Variety in placental shape: when does it originate? Placenta. 2012; 33, 164–170. 42. Naglieri J, Goldstein S. Understanding the strengths and weaknesses of intelligence and achievement tests. In Practitioner’s Guide to Assessing Intelligence and Achievement (eds. Naglieri J, Goldstein S), 2009; pp. 3–10. John Wiley and Sons: Hoboken, NJ. 43. Zacchigna S, Ruiz de Almodovar C, Carmeliet P. Similarities between angiogenesis and neural development: what small animal models can tell us. Curr Top Dev Biol. 2008; 80, 1–55. 44. Eichmann A, Le Noble F, Autiero M, Carmeliet P. Guidance of vascular and neural network formation. Curr Opin Neurobiol. 2005; 15, 108–115. 45. Autiero M, De Smet F, Claes F, Carmeliet P. Role of neural guidance signals in blood vessel navigation. Cardiovasc Res. 2005; 65, 629–638.

Placental measurements associated with IQ at age 7 years 46. Davies JA. Do different branching epithelia use a conserved developmental mechanism? Bioessays. 2002; 24, 937–948. 47. Patel N, Sharpe PT, Miletich I. Coordination of epithelial branching and salivary gland lumen formation by Wnt and FGF signals. Dev Biol. 2011; 358, 156–167. 48. Mayhew TM. Fetoplacental angiogenesis during gestation is biphasic, longitudinal and occurs by proliferation and remodelling of vascular endothelial cells. Placenta. 2002; 23, 742–750. 49. Benirshcke K, Kaufmann P, Baergen R. Fetoplacental angiogenesis as the driving force for villous development. In The Human Placenta, 5th edn (eds. Benirschke K, Kaufman P), 2006; pp. 146–154. Springer, New York. 50. Kingdom J, Huppertz B, Seaward G, Kaufmann P. Development of the placental villous tree and its consequences for fetal growth. Eur J Obstet Gynecol Reprod Biol. 2000; 92, 35–43. 51. Storkebaum E, Lambrechts D, Carmeliet P. VEGF: once regarded as a specific angiogenic factor, now implicated in neuroprotection. Bioessays. 2004; 26, 943–954. 52. Hu Y, Wang Y-d, Guo T, et al. Identification of brain-derived neurotrophic factor as a novel angiogenic protein in multiple myeloma. Cancer Genet Cytogenet. 2007; 178, 1–10. 53. Li Q, Ford MC, Lavik EB, Madri JA. Modeling the neurovascular niche: VEGF- and BDNF-mediated cross-talk between neural stem cells and endothelial cells: an in vitro study. J Neurosci Res. 2006; 84, 1656–1668. 54. Newton SS, Duman RS. Regulation of neurogenesis and angiogenesis in depression. Curr Neurovasc Res. 2004; 1, 261–267. 55. Kutcher ME, Klagsbrun M, Mamluk R. VEGF is required for the maintenance of dorsal root ganglia blood vessels but not neurons during development. FASEB J. 2004; 18, 1952–1954. 56. Van Den Bosch L, Storkebaum E, Vleminckx V, et al. Effects of vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) on motor neuron degeneration. Neurobiol Dis. 2004; 17, 21–28. 57. Carmeliet P, Storkebaum E. Vascular and neuronal effects of VEGF in the nervous system: implications for neurological disorders. Semin Cell Dev Biol. 2002; 13, 39–53. 58. Jin K, Zhu Y, Sun Y, et al. Vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF) stimulates neurogenesis in vitro and in vivo. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2002; 99, 11946–11950.

197

59. Jin KL, Mao XO, Greenberg DA. Vascular endothelial growth factor: direct neuroprotective effect in in vitro ischemia. Proc Natl Acad Sci U S A. 2000; 97, 10242–10247. 60. Louissaint Jr A, Rao S, Leventhal C, Goldman SA. Coordinated interaction of neurogenesis and angiogenesis in the adult songbird brain. Neuron. 2002; 34, 945–960. 61. Pitzer MR, Sortwell CE, Daley BF, et al. Angiogenic and neurotrophic effects of vascular endothelial growth factor (VEGF165): studies of grafted and cultured embryonic ventral mesencephalic cells. Exp Neurol. 2003; 182, 435–445. 62. Rosenstein JM, Mani N, Khaibullina A, Krum JM. Neurotrophic effects of vascular endothelial growth factor on organotypic cortical explants and primary cortical neurons. J Neurosci. 2003; 23, 11036–11044. 63. Sondell M, Lundborg G, Kanje M. Vascular endothelial growth factor has neurotrophic activity and stimulates axonal outgrowth, enhancing cell survival and Schwann cell proliferation in the peripheral nervous system. J Neurosci. 1999; 19, 5731–5740. 64. Sondell M, Sundler F, Kanje M. Vascular endothelial growth factor is a neurotrophic factor which stimulates axonal outgrowth through the flk-1 receptor. Eur J Neurosci. 2000; 12, 4243–4254. 65. Reynolds LP, Borowicz PP, Caton JS, et al. Uteroplacental vascular development and placental function: an update. Int J Dev Biol. 2010; 54, 355–366. 66. Helmestam M, Stavreus-Evers A, Olovsson M. Cadmium chloride alters mRNA levels of angiogenesis related genes in primary human endometrial endothelial cells grown in vitro. Reprod Toxicol (Elmsford, N Y). 2010; 30, 370–376. 67. Thaxton JE, Sharma S. Interleukin-10: a multi-faceted agent of pregnancy. Am J Reprod Immunol (New York, N Y). 1989; 63, 482–491. 68. Giudice LC. Microarray expression profiling reveals candidate genes for human uterine receptivity. Am J Pharmacogenomics. 2004; 4, 299–312. 69. Yampolsky M, CM S, Shlakhter O, et al. Variable placental thickness affects placental functional efficiency independent of other placental shape abnormalities. J Dev Orig Health Dis. 2011; 2, 205–211.

Placental measurements associated with intelligence quotient at age 7 years.

We hypothesized that placental villous branching that is measured by disk chorionic plate expansion and disk thickness is correlated with factors also...
104KB Sizes 2 Downloads 5 Views