Brain Stimulation 7 (2014) 793e799

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Brain Stimulation journal homepage: www.brainstimjrnl.com

Transcranial Oscillatory Direct Current Stimulation During Sleep Improves Declarative Memory Consolidation in Children With Attention-deficit/hyperactivity Disorder to a Level Comparable to Healthy Controls Alexander Prehn-Kristensen a, *, Manuel Munz a, Robert Göder b, Ines Wilhelm c, d, Katharina Korr a, Wiebke Vahl a, Christian D. Wiesner a, Lioba Baving a a

Department of Child and Adolescent Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Center for Integrative Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Germany Department of Psychiatry and Psychotherapy, Center for Integrative Psychiatry, School of Medicine, Christian-Albrechts-University Kiel, Germany Institute of Medical Psychology and Behavioural Neurobiology, University of Tübingen, Germany d Child Development Center, University Children’s Hospital Zürich, Switzerland b c

a r t i c l e i n f o

a b s t r a c t

Article history: Received 5 June 2014 Received in revised form 17 July 2014 Accepted 17 July 2014 Available online 19 August 2014

Background: Slow oscillations ( .4). Polysomnograhic data The ANOVA of SO power during post-stimulation intervals in ADHD did not reveal any effects (P < .09). However, when the analysis was based only on sleep stage 4 epochs, there was a main effect of STIM, indicating that the SO activity in sleep stage 4 was significantly enhanced after tDCS (157  17.4) compared to the sham condition [134  12.7; F(1,11) ¼ 7.3, P ¼ .02; see also Fig. 3]. The main effect POSITION [F(7,77) ¼ 12.8, P > .001] reflects that SO power was not distributed equally over the scalp, however the interaction STIM  POSITION was not significant [F(7,77) ¼ .49, P ¼ .833]. All macro sleep parameters (TIB, TST, sleep efficiency, and sleep stage durations) did not differ between stimulation and sham night (P > .3; see Table S1 in Supplement). Likewise, there were no differences in macro sleep parameters between ADHD during sham night and healthy children (P > .5; see Table S1 in Supplement). Table 2 Results of memory performance. ADHD

Controls

Sham mean Stimulation Stim vs. (SEM) mean (SEM) sham P Learning 67.5 (1.7) Retrieval 57.4 (3.0) Learning e 10.2 (2.4)a retrieval

65.6 (1.6) 66.2 (3.0) 0.5 (2.6)

.502 .076 .004b

SEM, standard error of means; Stim, stimulation. a different from zero (P ¼ .001). b one-tailed.

Controls Controls vs. sham vs. stim

Mean (SEM) 68.3 (3.1) 66.0 (3.1) 2.3 (3.4)

P

P

.815 .061 .036b

.433 .954 .520

A. Prehn-Kristensen et al. / Brain Stimulation 7 (2014) 793e799

797

Figure 2. Memory performance; * P < .036 (one-tailed); ** P ¼ .004 (one-tailed); *** P ¼ .001.

Control variables In children with ADHD, there were no significant main effects or interactions of STIM and SESSION with respect to mood ratings (SAM; P > .1) or digit-span performance (P > .1; for descriptive information see Table S2 in Supplement). Children with ADHD, in general, felt less alert before encoding compared to the retrieval session [F(1,11) ¼ 7.3, P ¼ .021], however, neither the main effect for STIM (P ¼ .6) nor the interaction STIM  SESSION was significant (P ¼ .9). In healthy children, there were no significant differences in mood/tiredness ratings or digit-span performance between encoding and retrieval sessions (P > .09). When comparing mood and alertness ratings between children with ADHD and healthy

controls no significant differences were found (P > .05). The digitspan performance was worse in children with ADHD than in healthy controls in both encoding sessions [stim: t(22) ¼ 2.2, P ¼ .041; sham: t(22) ¼ 2.2, P ¼ .041] and by trend during stim retrieval session [stim: t(22) ¼ 1.8, P ¼ .084; sham: t(22) ¼ .84, P ¼ .386]. Discussion In this study we investigated whether an external enhancement of slow oscillations (SO) by transcranial oscillating direct current stimulation (toDCS) could elevate sleep-dependent memory in children with ADHD to the level of healthy controls. While children

Figure 3. Difference in mean SO power (slow oscillations; 0.6e1.1 Hz) between 1-min intervals after stimulation and comparable intervals during sham night (left panel); arrows indicate location of stimulation; differences in SO power averaged over all electrodes (right panel); Stim, stimulation; * P ¼ .02.

798

A. Prehn-Kristensen et al. / Brain Stimulation 7 (2014) 793e799

with ADHD during sham condition showed worse memory performance than healthy controls, this memory deficit vanished after applying toDCS during sleep. Children and adults suffering from ADHD are known to display long-term memory deficits [9,45] which are often ascribed to deviant encoding rather than to problems during consolidation or retrieval [6]. Studies concerning long-term memory performance, however, often focused on memory encoding and retrieval on the same day, thus neglecting the supporting role of sleep in memory performance. In our previous studies, we controlled for sleep/wake states and observed deficits in sleep-dependent consolidation of long-term declarative memory (picture recognition) in ADHD. Most strikingly, reduced memory performance in ADHD after sleep was linked to SO activity during sleep [34,37]. As shown by Marshall and colleagues, frontally applied anodal toDCS in healthy adults can remarkably improve the sleep-dependent consolidation of declarative memory [32,33]. SO synchronizes hippocampal sharp wave ripple activity and thus fosters the integration of newly encoded hippocampus-related memories into already existing memory networks located in the neocortex [27,31,46]. Children suffering from ADHD are suspected to display less frontal slow wave activity during sleep [35]. Therefore, the external induction of frontal SO by toDCS in our study might have superimposed deficient SO functionality during sleep and thereby normalized sleepdependent memory consolidation in children with ADHD. Since the ethic committee did not support the proposal to stimulate healthy children by toDCS, children of the control group performed the experimental tasks at home and also slept in their own bed. This might lead to the concern that experimental sessions were not comparable between the two groups. Indeed, based on the findings that healthy children display worse cognitive performance at home than under laboratory conditions [47], differences between healthy children and children with ADHD in memory performance might have been even underestimated. To ensure that the home environment and laboratory conditions were comparable as much as possible, parents of control children were instructed to provide a quiet, room with an unstimulating workplace and to take care that no disturbing events happened during the sessions. Moreover, by introducing a learning criterion (60% correctly identified pair locations), we made sure that patients and healthy children ended the encoding session on a comparable level of encoded items. In the same way patients and healthy children during sham night did not differ in sleep length, efficiency and architecture, or in self-rated tiredness or mood. Therefore, we doubt that differences in memory performance can be attributed to different experimental conditions. The therapeutic benefit of brain stimulation by modulating cortical excitability and restoring neuronal networks in psychiatric patients becomes more and more evident [48e50]. For example, in the case of depression the positive effect of tDCS can last for weeks [51]. This, however, might lead to the concern that stimulation did not boost sleep-dependent memory consolidation itself but rather induced a non-specific cognitive enhancement supporting retrieval performance on the next morning. Please note that only memory performances but not the performance on any control variable (mood, alertness, or working memory performance) was affected by the stimulation. Moreover, our stimulation method did not follow the usual tDCS protocols for the treatment of clinical symptoms during daytime as reviewed by [48,49]: a) in comparison to most other studies, we applied DC only in one single stimulation session; b) the applied current strength in our study was significantly lower than in other studies (0.25 mA vs. 1e2 mA); and c) DC stimulation in our study was not constant above 1 mA (or higher) but oscillating, ranging from zero to a maximum of 0.25 mA. In addition, there is evidence that toDCS only at 0.75 Hz (not at 5 Hz)

and applied only during sleep leads to an increase of memory consolidation [32,52,53]. In the same way, Mölle and colleagues replicated a comparable memory-boosting effect by using acoustic instead of electrical stimulation: every time an SO wave (

hyperactivity disorder to a level comparable to healthy controls.

Slow oscillations (...
790KB Sizes 0 Downloads 8 Views