rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

Research

Greater than the sum of its parts? Modelling population contact and interaction of cultural repertoires Nicole Creanza1,†, Oren Kolodny2,† and Marcus W. Feldman2 1 2

Cite this article: Creanza N, Kolodny O, Feldman MW. 2017 Greater than the sum of its parts? Modelling population contact and interaction of cultural repertoires. J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171. http://dx.doi.org/10.1098/rsif.2017.0171

Received: 7 March 2017 Accepted: 6 April 2017

Subject Category: Life Sciences – Mathematics interface Subject Areas: evolution Keywords: cultural accumulation, migration, population structure, connectivity, archaeology

Authors for correspondence: Nicole Creanza e-mail: [email protected] Oren Kolodny e-mail: [email protected]



These authors contributed equally to this study. Electronic supplementary material is available online at https://dx.doi.org/10.6084/m9. figshare.c.3744347.

Department of Biological Sciences, Vanderbilt University, Nashville, TN 37235-1634, USA Department of Biology, Stanford University, Stanford, CA 94305, USA NC, 0000-0001-8821-7383 Evidence for interactions between populations plays a prominent role in the reconstruction of historical and prehistoric human dynamics; these interactions are usually interpreted to reflect cultural practices or demographic processes. The sharp increase in long-distance transportation of lithic material between the Middle and Upper Palaeolithic, for example, is seen as a manifestation of the cultural revolution that defined the transition between these epochs. Here, we propose that population interaction is not only a reflection of cultural change but also a potential driver of it. We explore the possible effects of inter-population migration on cultural evolution when migrating individuals possess core technological knowledge from their original population. Using a computational framework of cultural evolution that incorporates realistic aspects of human innovation processes, we show that migration can lead to a range of outcomes, including punctuated but transient increases in cultural complexity, an increase of cultural complexity to an elevated steady state and the emergence of a positive feedback loop that drives ongoing acceleration in cultural accumulation. Our findings suggest that population contact may have played a crucial role in the evolution of hominin cultures and propose explanations for observations of Palaeolithic cultural change whose interpretations have been hotly debated.

1. Introduction Long-distance hominid mobility, which probably correlates with inter-population connectivity, can be inferred from various aspects of the archaeological record; for example, transportation of material and artefacts over distances greater than 100 km occurred sporadically in the Middle Palaeolithic and regularly in the Upper Palaeolithic [1]. This feature of the Upper Palaeolithic revolution is usually attributed to demographic processes, changes in subsistence strategies or other cultural shifts [1,2]. We suggest that inter-population connectivity may be more than a reflection of cultural advancement: it may have been critical in driving such change. In this study, we explore the cultural dynamics that may result from population contact. Connectivity within and between populations has been proposed, in theoretical and anthropological studies, to dramatically influence cultural evolution [3– 9]. An experimental human-interaction study showed that groups produce more complex artefacts than individuals acting alone [10], and several anthropological studies and evolutionary models suggest a relationship between group size and technological complexity (e.g. [11 –13]). In the workplace, innovations appear more often when members of different groups interact [14]. Further, experimental groups that independently accumulated traits and then combined their knowledge made successful innovative combinations not observed in fully connected groups [15]. Similarly, a recent model simulated

& 2017 The Authors. Published by the Royal Society under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License http://creativecommons.org/licenses/by/4.0/, which permits unrestricted use, provided the original author and source are credited.

2. The model We extend the model of Kolodny et al. [28] to include multiple populations whose cultures independently innovate and evolve. We simulate the effects of migration and cultural interactions between these populations. In the model, we assume that each individual has some probability of innovating and of migrating, so the overall rates of innovation and migration in the population are proportional to population size. Similarly, we assume that cultural traits are more susceptible to loss when fewer people know them, so the overall rate of cultural loss is inversely related to population size. Finally, we assume that certain rare innovations, such as those that increase the food supply, can increase carrying capacity and thus affect population size. Although one cannot generalize to all human populations from a single model, a body of empirical and theoretical literature supports these assumptions (e.g. [11,13,30,34 –38,45]). In the model, three interacting processes contribute to human tool innovation, as in [28]. The first process produces ground-breaking large-scale innovations, or lucky leaps, which occur with probability Plucky per individual per time step. Each lucky leap innovation facilitates two other tool innovation processes. First, a number of tools are made useful by each lucky leap; these are termed toolkit innovations. There are L toolkit innovations associated with each lucky leap, where L is sampled from a uniform distribution U(1,11). Lucky leap innovations can also combine with other lucky leap innovations to produce innovative combinations, which are useful to the population with probability PcombUseful. With only lucky leaps allowed to combine, this relatively conservative combination scheme represents the notion that ground-breaking ideas are often widely applicable to other existing technologies. For simplicity, we assume that all potentially useful combinations and toolkit tools are innovated immediately upon the lucky leap’s invention, which is equivalent to the assumption that an individual tests more than one combination per time step.

2

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

To study the effects of inter-population contact on cultural dynamics, we develop a theoretical framework that considers jointly cultural contact, innovation and modifiers of biological carrying capacity. Here, populations innovate and accumulate cultural traits independently, and individuals migrate between populations bringing the core technologies invented in their original population, which facilitates cultural change. In addition, technologies can be combined to form new tools, and the many novel combinations that become possible following a migration event may potentially trigger a burst of innovations. As in real-life human cultures, the potential number of cultural traits in our model is theoretically unlimited. By considering the effects of both population size and population structure on cultural accumulation, our model addresses human experimental and archaeological data. In particular, our model suggests that large-scale punctuation in the archaeological record can result from an increase in inter-population connectivity. We explore the possibility that cultural contact is a primary driver of rapid cultural change and characterize patterns that would emerge under different migration regimes.

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

contact between populations with a continuum of mobility strategies, from remaining near a home base to constantly moving, with no home base [16]; the results suggested that intermediate strategies, which might ensure both regular contact with new populations and enough contact time to accurately transmit information, maximized cultural transmission across population boundaries. From their archaeological analyses, Stiner & Kuhn [17] suggested that the connectedness of the Upper Palaeolithic could have stabilized technological volatility, decreasing risk and increasing demographic robustness. Along the same lines, Hovers & Belfer-Cohen [18] proposed that population interconnectedness prevents local loss of culture, and that the Middle Palaeolithic record reflects a pattern of cultural extinction and re-invention, stemming from instability of transmission networks. These empirical and theoretical studies suggest that modelling the effect of inter-population interactions on overall cultural complexity may be useful in interpreting the archaeological record of hominid culture. One of the apparent features of the time trajectory of culture is that it includes periods of relative stasis that are separated by bursts of cultural accumulation; these increases can differ in both time scale and magnitude [19– 22]. Previous explanations for punctuations in the archaeological record have invoked a cultural reaction to such factors as genetic/ cognitive change in hominids or environmental change that alters the population’s cultural steady state [21,23– 27]. In a previous study, we proposed that independent innovation processes can explain cultural bursts: if a cultural advance facilitates associated innovations and novel trait combinations, then a purely cultural mechanism can trigger a cascade of related innovations and punctuated cultural bursts [28]. An alternative driver of punctuation could be sudden changes in the parameters of cultural evolution, such as those brought about by modification of the biological carrying capacity, the number of individuals that the available resources can support [29]. Thus, a cultural trait, for example a tool or practice related to agriculture [30,31], could increase food availability and the biological carrying capacity. The resulting population growth might correspond to an increased cultural repertoire, as predicted by experiments, some cross-cultural analyses and cultural–evolutionary models [11–13,32–40]. Notably, these carrying-capacity-altering cultural shifts can lead to much greater cultural accumulation [29] than those induced by a cascade of related innovations in the model of [28]. Most models of cultural evolution consider the spread of existing traits, but only a handful explicitly model the innovation processes that underlie the origin of new traits [28,41,42]. Some models have addressed the effect of population structure; for example, migration among subpopulations may affect the population’s cultural diversity [37,40,43] and the accumulation of errors [43]. Further, migration among subpopulations could affect the cultural repertoire, both because cultural loss is less likely with access to more cultural models [12] and because rare innovations are more likely to spread throughout the population [44]. In [44], migration and population size had a greater effect on pre-equilibrium dynamics than on the cultural equilibrium of a population, but this analysis examined the skill level of a finite cultural repertoire as opposed to additions to a potentially limitless cultural repertoire.

ð2:1Þ

ntoolkit ¼

N 2  Plucky  kLl 2  PSpontLoss

ð2:2Þ

and

ncomb ¼

N 4  P2lucky  PCombUseful 2  P2SpontLoss

,

ð2:3Þ

where N is the population size and kLl is the mean number of toolkit innovations associated with each lucky leap tool. Here, we extend this framework [28] to include the effects of population size and cultural contact. First, we implement

3

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

N 2  Plucky , PSpontLoss

nlucky ¼

multiple simulations of the model simultaneously, generating independent populations that undergo innovation and cultural evolution. (The present framework can simulate many interacting populations with qualitatively similar results; figures 1–5 display two or three populations for ease of visualization.) Then, individuals migrate between populations with probability Pmigrate per time step. An individual enters a new population carrying with it some fraction, fmigrant, of the full repertoire of core technologies in its original population, i.e. its repertoire of lucky leap innovations and their associated toolkits. We explored the dynamics of population-level subdivision of knowledge in [28]; for simplicity in this study, in figures 1 –4 we set fmigrant ¼ 1, i.e. each migrant carries its originating population’s full cultural repertoire; this does not influence the results qualitatively. Following migration, the migrant-receiving population can test many potential combinations between its existing lucky leaps and the newly arrived lucky leaps. Each of these potential combinations is useful with probability PcombUseful, which we set equal to 1 in the following simulations to illustrate the potential scale of the effects of combining cultures. In reality, cultural trait combinations are not necessarily useful but are also not restricted to combinations of large-scale lucky leap innovations. To consider separately the effects of migration and the effects of changes in population size, we assume that a migration event occurs according to a Moran model [47], with no change in size of either population: the migrant can be thought of as replacing a randomly chosen individual who died in the population it is joining, and its place in its original population is filled by a newborn individual. Lucky leap innovations that occur independently in different populations are assumed to be distinct, so two populations will initially have no tools in common. An individual from population 2 who joins population 1 brings population 2’s lucky leap innovations and their associated toolkits. Once this occurs, a lucky leap that originated in population 2 can be combined with lucky leaps in population 1. For example, combining tool A from population 1 with tool B that originated in population 2 would lead to the combined tool AB. Also, all combinations of innovations A and B are identical to one another, even if the process that combined them occurred independently in different populations or occurred in a different order (BA ¼ AB). In human history, fortuitous innovations enabled increases in carrying capacity, resulting in population growth [30,31], which then likely facilitated larger cultural repertoires. We include this possibility in our model: with probability PIncreaseCarryingCapacity, each new combination increases the biological carrying capacity of the population. If this stochastic event occurs at time t, the population size (N) increases by a factor of C, sampled from a uniform distribution U(1.1,1.2): Ntþ1 ¼ Nt . C. As PIncreaseCarryingCapacity acts on each new combination, carrying-capacity-altering traits are likely to arise when two cultures are connected by migration. We hypothesize that carrying-capacity-altering traits are resistant to cultural loss because the effects of losing the behaviour (for example, less available food) will quickly put pressure on the population to reintroduce it. Thus, carrying-capacity-altering combinations are placed in a distinct category of tools with their own loss probability, which we set to zero in the results presented below.

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

Several models represent cultural traits as skills and track variation in individual skill levels [12,13,35,44], and others track the presence of traits in individual cultural repertoires [34,37,40,46]; in these studies, transmission between individuals is explicitly modelled, and cultural complexity increases with population size. Here, we build upon these findings to simplify the transmission process: we track the population-level presence of traits, as in [36,38], and we assign a probability that a trait will arise and spread in the population rather than focusing on individual-level transmission processes. Finally, at each time step, tools may be stochastically lost due to drift. Because the rate of cultural loss is likely to decrease as the population grows [13], we scale the loss parameter, PSpontLoss, by the population size: PSpontLoss/N. This loss probability encapsulates numerous possible loss processes, including failed transmission between individuals and fluctuating trait frequencies that may decrease to extinction by chance. In reality, a certain trait’s probability of loss depends on many factors, including its ease of transmission, effect on biological fitness and usefulness in the current environment [28]; for simplicity, we use the same probability of loss for all traits. (A similar approximation is found in [36] and the electronic supplementary material of [38], where agent-based transmission of knowledge is taken into account.) This rate of stochastic trait loss is a useful first approximation that captures how loss might scale with population size and how even important traits can be lost [13]. When a lucky leap tool is lost, the toolkit and combination tools associated with it are also lost. Toolkit innovations and combination tools, however, can be individually lost without affecting other tools. These tools may also be re-invented in later time steps, if the lucky leap innovations with which they were associated remain in the population. This occurs with toolkit and combination tools, respectively, with probabilities PToolkit and PCombine per individual per time step. In these agent-based stochastic simulations, each process occurs with a given probability per time step; thus, each run of the stochastic simulation is unique. In the electronic supplementary material, we also give equations for the expected effect of each process under simplifying assumptions. The framework outlined above is sufficient to produce punctuated bursts of innovations after periods of stasis, as a lucky leap innovation can facilitate the relatively rapid addition of combinations and toolkit innovations [28]. When tools can be lost as well as added, the mean number of each type of tool (nlucky, ntoolkit, ncomb) approaches a steady state (derived in the electronic supplementary material):

(a)

(b)

N = 50

4

N = 25

total number of tools

3000 40 000

40 000

1500 30 000

30 000

0 0 20 000

20 000

10 000

10 000

1000

0

2000 time step

3000

4000

0

0

1000

2000 time step

3000

500

4000

Figure 1. The tool repertoire size of a population of size 2N (a) is much larger than the sum of two populations of size N (b). In this example, a population has approximately 40 000 tools, whereas the same population divided into two disconnected subpopulations has approximately 8000 tools at steady state. In (b), one population’s cultural trajectory is shown. In both panels, red indicates lucky leaps (visible at the bottom of the inset of (b)), orange indicates toolkit innovations and yellow indicates combination tools. Other parameters in (a) populations ¼ 1, N ¼ 50, Plucky ¼ 0.08, PcombUseful ¼ 1, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.08, Pmigrate ¼ 0; (b) populations ¼ 2, N ¼ 25, Plucky ¼ 0.08, PcombUseful ¼ 1, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.08, Pmigrate ¼ 0.

total number of tools

10 000 8000 6000 4000 2000 0 0

500

1000

1500

2000

2500 time step

3000

3500

4000

4500

5000

Figure 2. The effect of rare migration on cultural repertoire size. For t ¼ 0 to t ¼ 2000, there is no migration (Pmigrate ¼ 0); after t ¼ 2000, migration is possible but very rare (Pmigrate ¼ 0.000025). One population’s cultural trajectory is shown. Migration events (red dots on the x-axis) represent the arrival of a new individual to the population. Following the initial burst of culture driven by the combinations between the existing tools and those introduced by migration, there is a gradual decay back to the steady state. Other parameters: populations ¼ 2, N ¼ 25, Plucky ¼ 0.08, PcombUseful ¼ 1, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.08.

3. Results 3.1. The effect of population size on the cultural repertoire As each individual may invent a novel tool with some probability, the rate of tool accumulation increases with population size. This accords with most models of cultural evolution, despite different approaches [12,37,39 –41,46,48]. Loss of tools in our model is not implemented explicitly as a result of failed cultural transmission. To approximate tool loss, we implement directly the main qualitative finding of previous models with explicit transmission processes: a tool’s probability of loss is inversely dependent on the population size, because additional tool users decrease the likelihood of failed transmission [28,36,38]. The combination of innovation and loss in our model leads to a nonlinear relationship between repertoire size and population size (equations (2.1)– (2.3)): repertoire size scales with N 2 for lucky leap and toolkit innovations and with N 4 for combination innovations. Slight variations of our model, such as different combination rules, would lead to somewhat different relationships between population size and repertoire size, but, qualitatively, the expected correlation is polynomial in N (see also [28]).

Because of this nonlinear dependence on population size, population subdivision has a dramatic effect on cultural repertoire size: a population of size 2N has a much higher cultural steady state in our model than the sum of two populations of size N (figure 1). As the relationship between N and repertoire size is highly sensitive to the details of the model, which is inevitably a gross simplification of reality, we do not attempt to fit our model’s numerical results to empirical data. However, linking the qualitative trends produced by our model with empirical findings can be useful. Our model’s prediction of a polynomial dependency of repertoire size on N implies that small differences in population size or connectivity can lead to previously underappreciated disproportionate differences in cultural complexity. This may help explain features of the transition from the Middle to the Upper Palaeolithic, as elaborated below.

3.2. The effect of rare migration on the cultural repertoire In our model, combining existing tools can produce innovations. As a result, many new combinations are suddenly possible after an initial migration event, and testing these new combinations leads to a rapid burst of innovations.

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

0

250

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

50 000

50 000

15 000

(a)

5

total number of tools

5000 0 0

1000

2000

3000

4000

5000

6000

7000

8000

1000

2000

3000

4000 time step

5000

6000

7000

8000

(b) 15 000 10 000

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

10 000

5000 0

Figure 3. The effect of frequent migration on cultural repertoire size. As in figure 2, Pmigrate ¼ 0 for t ¼ 0 to t ¼ 2000; after t ¼ 2000, Pmigrate ¼ 0.0001 in (a) and Pmigrate ¼ 0.4 in (b). Each panel illustrates one population’s cultural trajectory. In (a), migration events are indicated by red dots on the x-axis; in (b), these events occur so frequently (more than once per time step) that the dots are individually indistinguishable. As the overall migration rate increases, the cultural repertoire does not return to the original steady state between migration events; thus, migration effectively elevates the cultural steady state of the population. Other parameters (a,b): populations ¼ 2, N ¼ 25, Plucky ¼ 0.08, PcombUseful ¼ 1, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.08.

However, as we assume that the population size remains constant after migration [47], the cultural steady state is also constant. Thus, after the initial burst of innovation, the cultural repertoire gradually decays to approach the original steady state (figure 2).

3.3. The effect of frequent migration on the cultural repertoire As the migration rate, Pmigrate, increases, the cultural repertoire of the receiving population does not have enough time between migration events to decay to the steady state (figure 3); thus, average cultural repertoire size increases. The effect of migration on the cultural steady state becomes more apparent as the migration rate increases: with more frequent migration, bursts of cultural accumulation no longer occur and a population has a relatively stable cultural repertoire that is substantially larger than the steady state predicted by its population size. This result accords with the findings of Powell et al. [12] regarding possible differences between world regions. Notably, in our model frequent migration between two populations of size N produces a total cultural repertoire that is smaller than that of an unstructured population of size 2N because the loss parameter, PSpontLoss, is still scaled by N and not 2N.

Interestingly, if migration is intermediate in frequency, populations may evolve while remaining culturally distinct: core technologies are transmitted, but without the combination tools that are associated with each, and cultural losses are stochastic; thus, the combinations that arise in different populations only partially overlap. Major innovations, such as those that increase carrying capacity, are very likely to spread between the populations and remain shared, because of their adaptive value (our model implements this via the assumption that carrying-capacity-altering innovations are not stochastically lost; see also [5,12]). These shared carrying capacity increases lead to populations of similar size and thus similar cultural complexity (equations (2.1)–(2.3)). For a while, separate populations could co-develop: changes in population sizes and cultural complexity would occur separately in each population, but with higher correlation in timing than expected for independent populations (figure 5a–c). Populations would only remain separate temporarily: because population growth increases overall migration rate, eventually migration occurs frequently enough to prevent significant differentiation between the cultures (figure 5d). Note that Pmigrate, an individual’s migration probability, changes in figure 5 at predetermined time steps, demonstrating the possible effect of sudden changes in migration rate.

4. Discussion 3.4. Migration and carrying capacity changes In our model, following migration, tools that arose in separate populations can combine, and each may, with some probability, be a tool that increases carrying capacity. When this happens, the cultural steady state also increases, leading to more step-wise accumulation of culture (figure 4a) instead of a burst-and-decay pattern (figure 2). Carrying-capacity-altering innovations could also initiate a feedback loop: when the carrying capacity changes, the population grows and both the cultural repertoire and the effective migration rate increase, which further increases the likelihood that other carryingcapacity-altering innovations occur, ratcheting the cultural steady state upward (figure 4b).

Human innovation is a multi-faceted process [42], but most models of cultural evolution primarily consider the transmission of existing cultural traits. To address this, we have proposed models that assess the role of interdependent innovation processes in causing cultural accumulation within a population [28,29]. However, recent experimental evidence underscores the importance of innovation via population interaction: groups with independently evolving cultural repertoires can produce useful new innovations by combining their existing innovations [15]. Here, we propose a fairly simple model that synthesizes these two research areas: multiple-independent populations undergo processes of innovation and cultural accumulation

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

0

(b)

50 000

6

80 000

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

total number of tools

(a)

40 000 60 000 30 000 40 000

20 000

20 000

10 000

0

0 1000

2000 time step

3000

0

1000

2000 time step

3000

Figure 4. The effect of frequent migration and changes in carrying capacity on cultural repertoire size. Pmigrate ¼ 0 for t ¼ 0 to t ¼ 2000; after t ¼ 2000, Pmigrate ¼ 0.0001 and PIncreaseCarryingCapacity ¼ 0.0001 in (a), and Pmigrate ¼ 0.005 and PIncreaseCarryingCapacity ¼ 0.0005 in (b). Each panel illustrates one population’s cultural trajectory. Red dots indicate migration events, and blue diamonds indicate the origin of innovations that trigger growth of carrying capacity. When migration is rare and innovations alter the carrying capacity relatively rarely, the cultural trajectory appears punctuated (a); changes to carrying capacity frequently occur following migration events due to the burst of new combinations that they induce. When migration is more frequent, innovations alter the capacity more often and the cultural repertoire increases rapidly without approaching a steady state (b). Other parameters (a,b): populations ¼ 2, N ¼ 25, Plucky ¼ 0.08, PcombUseful ¼ 1, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.08. Each increase in carrying capacity (blue diamonds) is by a factor of between 1.1 and 1.2; by the end of the simulation shown, the population in panel (a) had reached N ¼ 54, and the population in panel (b) reached N ¼ 65. separately, and migration allows the populations’ cultural repertoires to be combined, producing additional innovations. Further, we consider that some of these novel cultural combinations might alter the biological carrying capacity of the population, causing population growth and a resulting increase in the cultural steady state, the population’s expected number of cultural traits. Our model produces five prominent patterns that appear to differ from those of most previous model-based studies. (i) We observe a polynomial relation between population size and cultural complexity, causing small changes in N to have disproportionate effects on repertoire size. (ii) We find that rare migration may lead to transient emergence of cultural complexity, which subsequently decays in small or relatively disconnected populations. (iii) Changes in migration rates may increase effective cultural population sizes with no change in local population sizes, potentially driving changes in cultural complexity. (iv) If culture affects carrying capacity or range expansion and if population size influences migration, a positive feedback loop may develop in which population growth, inter-population contact and cultural complexity interact. Such a feedback loop could have driven the demographic and cultural explosion that occurred in Eurasia shortly after the Middle to Upper Palaeolithic transition, as these three components are prominent characteristics of this transition [1,2,49 –53]. Cultural innovations, such as inventions that change subsistence patterns or facilitate expansion to previously uninhabitable climates, could have driven population increases; support for both elements is found in the archaeological record [2,54–61]. (v) Complex cultural patterns may arise when multiple populations interact and exchange knowledge at intermediate frequencies, potentially driving one another towards related, but non-identical, trajectories of population growth and increased cultural complexity. These dynamics are transient if they subsequently increase migration, which eventually links the populations and homogenizes their cultures. A single narrative is unlikely to accommodate the full range of archaeological observations regarding cultural evolution in the Palaeolithic. Instead, we propose that the patterns derived from our model may contribute to attempts to

understand the archaeological record. By predicting a nonlinear relationship between population size and cultural repertoire, our model raises the possibility that undetectable increases in population size could drive disproportionately large changes in cultural complexity; alternatively, an increase in connectivity among populations, without population growth, could increase effective cultural population size and lead to cultural transition. For example, although anatomically modern humans evolved in Africa approximately 160–200 ka [62–64], behavioural modernity occurred significantly later, with the full ‘package’ of cultural traits characteristic of the Upper Palaeolithic occurring only approximately 45 ka in the Levant, Europe, Western Asia and perhaps East/South Africa [2,65–69]. Estimates based on genetic and archaeological evidence indicate that both population sizes and densities increased in these regions near this time [49,70]. Our model predicts a nonlinear relationship between population size and cultural complexity, which suggests that cultural evolutionary dynamics could have driven the transition to behavioural modernity; thus, invoking biological change to explain this transition (as in [71,72]) is unnecessary. Qualitatively similar results and interpretations, relating cultural complexity to population size and migration in the transition to the Upper Palaeolithic, have been suggested by Powell et al. [12]. In addition, archaeological evidence does not unequivocally support significant population growth in Africa 50–45 ka [73,74], which has generated criticism towards attributing behavioural modernity’s emergence to population size [11,75,76]. Our model’s prediction that small changes in population sizes or migration patterns could drive large cultural change may contribute to this discussion. Further, a major characteristic of the Upper Palaeolithic revolution is the dramatic increase in the distance of material and artefact transportation [1,2,77,78]. As an increase in contact can effectively connect populations, thus forming a single metapopulation with a larger cultural repertoire, our results suggest that connectivity could have been a major driver of this cultural revolution and not just one of its outcomes (see also [43], which analyses the combined effect of connectedness with demographic fluctuation and local extinction).

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

0

(a) 8000 6000 4000 2000 0 400

600

700

800

900

500

600

700

800

900

500

600

700

800

900

500

600

700

800

900

total number of tools

10 000 8000 6000 4000 2000 0 400

(c) total number of tools

10 000 8000 6000 4000 2000 0

(d) proportion of tools

400 1.0 0.8 0.6 0.4 0.2 0 400

time step

Figure 5. Co-development of partially connected populations. Panels (a – c) show cultural dynamics in three contemporaneous populations (N1 ¼ N2 ¼ N3 ¼ 25; colour scheme as in previous figures). Panel (d ) shows the fraction of cultural overlap of combination tools among populations: the mean fraction of tools in each population that are unique to that population (blue), the mean fraction of combination tools that are shared with one other population (cyan), and the mean fraction of combination tools that are common to all three populations (yellow). Each population’s culture is unique (Pmigrate ¼ 0) until t ¼ 500, and cultural complexity is near steady state for long periods of time. From t ¼ 500 to t ¼ 800, Pmigrate ¼ 0.0004. During this phase, partially coordinated cultural change occurs, while each population remains culturally distinct: migration events (red dots) drive punctuated increases in cultural complexity (each migrant introduces into the new population each of the core technologies from its original population with probability fmigrant ¼ 0.3; the new combination tools that become possible drive the increase in cultural complexity), and inventions that increase biological carrying capacity spread quickly (blue dots, (a – c)). Overall repertoire sizes increase in all populations by similar orders of magnitude, while cultural overlap of combination tools increases gradually, but with a significant fraction of each population’s repertoire remaining unique (d ). At t ¼ 800, Pmigrate is increased to 0.04. This more frequent migration leads to a state reminiscent of a single large population, driving overall cultural repertoire sizes upwards sharply (a – c) and effectively near-homogenizing the populations’ cultures (d ). Other parameters (a – d): PIncreaseCarryingCapacity ¼ 0.01, Plucky ¼ 0.02, PSpontLoss ¼ 0.02. Every increase in biological carrying capacity (blue dots) is by a factor of between 1.1 and 1.2, leading the populations to increase by the end of the simulation from N1 ¼ N2 ¼ N3 ¼ 25 to N1 ¼ 36, N2 ¼ 39 and N3 ¼ 32.

7

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

500

(b)

A nonlinear relationship between population size and cultural complexity also provides a possible explanation for the occurrence of full behavioural modernity only among modern humans: estimates from genetic diversity suggest that Neanderthals had a threefold smaller effective population size than modern humans [79 –81]. Neanderthals and modern humans may have had similar cognitive and physical capacity for behavioural modernity [82–85], yet behavioural modernity only occurs in humans following the Neanderthal replacement [52,86]. In our model, when an individual migrates to a new population, the receiving population experiences a cultural burst because many novel combinations of innovations are suddenly possible. However, when migration is very rare, the population size, and thus the cultural steady state, remains constant, and the receiving population experiences a gradual decay to its original steady-state repertoire size (figure 2). This decay in the cultural repertoire after the initial acquisition of imported knowledge has precedents in the anthropological literature: even beneficial cultural traits from one population do not necessarily spread in another [87,88], potentially because of conflicting cultural norms [88] or language barriers [89]. Our model demonstrates that these complex cultural dynamics might occur, without making assumptions about social networks or transmission rules. A population’s migration rate may depend on numerous factors, including geographical boundaries, subsistence strategies and cultural practices, which may help explain the patchy appearance and disappearance of stone tool technocomplexes and other cultural practices during the Lower and Middle Palaeolithic [2,18,90]. More frequent contact between two populations would effectively increase the tool repertoire at steady state, as migrants may reintroduce cultural traits before the receiving population’s repertoire can fully decay to its steady state (figure 3). Migration can thus foster an elevated level of culture, either because migration occurs regularly or because the cultural steady state increases by some other mechanism. We explore one such mechanism by considering cultural traits that alter the biological carrying capacity. Throughout history and prehistory, cultural innovations have enabled human populations to extract more resources from their habitat, probably leading to population growth and subsequent increase of the cultural repertoire. In our model, when independent cultures come into contact, we assume that with small probability an innovative combination of their traits will increase the biological carrying capacity. The increased carrying capacity allows population growth, in turn elevating the cultural steady state. Thus, after a carrying-capacity-altering innovation occurs, the contact-induced burst of innovative combinations persists instead of decaying (figure 4). The results in figure 5 provide a possible explanation for one of the hotly debated observations in the transition between the Middle and Upper Palaeolithic in Europe: the transient prosperity of many cultures within a relatively short time span near the Middle –Upper Palaeolithic transition, such as the Uluzzian, Bachokirian, Chaˆtelperronian, Bohunician and Proto-Aurignacian [91 –95], which were distinct yet shared a number of characteristics that set them all apart from Middle Palaeolithic cultures [51,86,94,96–102]. This relatively sudden appearance of multiple distinct complex cultures with shared features is unlikely to be a coincidence. It could have been brought about by gradual

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

total number of tools

10 000

Data accessibility. The simulation code is available at https://github. com/CreanzaLab/CulturalMigrationAndConnectivity. Authors’ contributions. O.K. and N.C. designed the study, performed and analysed simulations, and interpreted results. O.K., N.C. and M.W.F. wrote the manuscript and approved publication. Competing interests. We have no competing interests.

Funding. This research was supported by the Stanford Center for Computational, Evolutionary, and Human Genomics and the John Templeton Foundation (47981). Acknowledgements. We are grateful to the ‘New Perspectives in Cultural Evolution’ workshop participants for helpful discussions.

References 1.

2.

3.

4.

5.

6.

7.

8.

Fe´blot-Augustins J. 2009 Revisiting European Upper Paleolithic raw material transfers: the demise of the cultural ecological paradigm? In Lithic materials and Paleolithic societies (eds B Adams, BS Blades), pp. 25 –46. Oxford, UK: Wiley-Blackwell. Bar-Yosef O. 2002 The Upper Paleolithic revolution. Annu. Rev. Anthropol. 31, 363 –393. (doi:10.1146/ annurev.anthro.31.040402.085416) Cavalli-Sforza LL, Feldman MW. 1981 Cultural transmission and evolution: a quantitative approach. Princeton, NJ: Princeton University Press. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 1985 Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago, IL: Chicago University Press. Boyd R, Richerson PJ. 2009 Voting with your feet: payoff biased migration and the evolution of group beneficial behavior. J. Theor. Biol. 257, 331–339. (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2008.12.007) Kobayashi Y, Ohtsuki H, Wakano JY. 2016 Population size vs. social connectedness—a gene-culture coevolutionary approach to cumulative cultural evolution. Theor. Popul. Biol. 111, 87–95. (doi:10. 1016/j.tpb.2016.07.001) Mesoudi A, Whiten A. 2008 The multiple roles of cultural transmission experiments in understanding human cultural evolution. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 363, 3489–3501. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2008.0129) Hill KR, Wood BM, Baggio J, Hurtado AM, Boyd RT. 2014 Hunter-gatherer inter-band interaction rates:

9.

10.

11.

12.

13.

14.

15.

implications for cumulative culture. PLoS ONE 9, e102806. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0102806) Anthony DW. 1990 Migration in archeology: the baby and the bathwater. Am. Anthropol. 92, 895 –914. (doi:10.1525/aa.1990.92.4.02a00030) Derex M, Boyd R. 2015 The foundations of the human cultural niche. Nat. Commun. 6, 8398. (doi:10.1038/ncomms9398) Kline MA, Boyd R. 2010 Population size predicts technological complexity in Oceania. Proc. R. Soc. B 277, 2559 – 2564. (doi:10.1098/rspb.2010. 0452) Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG. 2009 Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 324, 1298–1301. (doi:10.1126/science.1170165) Henrich J. 2004 Demography and cultural evolution: how adaptive cultural processes can produce maladaptive losses: the Tasmanian case. Am. Antiq. 69, 197– 214. (doi:10.2307/4128416) Bosetti V, Cattaneo C, Verdolini E, Bosetti V, Cattaneo C, Verdolini E. 2012 Migration, cultural diversity and innovation: a European perspective. FEEM Work. Pap. 69, 37. (doi:10.2139/ssrn. 2162836) Derex M, Boyd R. 2016 Partial connectivity increases cultural accumulation within groups. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci USA. 113, 2982– 2987. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 1518798113)

16. Perreault C, Brantingham PJ. 2011 Mobility-driven cultural transmission along the forager-collector continuum. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 30, 62 –68. (doi:10.1016/j.jaa.2010.10.003) 17. Stiner MC, Kuhn SL. 2006 Changes in the ‘Connectedness’ and resilience of Paleolithic societies in Mediterranean ecosystems. Hum. Ecol. 34, 693 –712. (doi:10.1007/s10745-006-9041-1) 18. Hovers E, Belfer-Cohen A. 2006 ‘Now you see it, now you don’t’—modern human behavior in the Middle Paleolithic. In Transitions before the transition: evolution and stability in the Middle Paleolithic and Middle Stone Age (eds E Hovers, SL Kuhn), pp. 295 –304. Berlin, Germany: Springer. 19. Aiyar S, Dalgaard C-J, Moav O. 2008 Technological progress and regress in pre-industrial times. J. Econ. Growth 13, 125– 144. (doi:10.1007/s10887-0089030-x) 20. Kuhn SL. 2012 Emergent patterns of creativity and innovation in early technologies. In Origins of human innovation and creativity (ed. S Elias), pp. 69 –88. Oxford, UK: Elsevier. 21. Klein RG. 2002 The dawn of human culture. New York, NY: John Wiley & Sons. 22. Lehman HC. 1947 The exponential increase of man’s cultural output. Soc. Forces 25, 281–290. (doi:10. 2307/3005665) 23. Ziegler M, Simon MH, Hall IR, Barker S, Stringer C, Zahn R. 2013 Development of Middle Stone Age

8

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

a cultural response, resulting in increased cultural accumulation. Here, we have explored two cultural factors that can provoke bursts of innovation: population contact via migration, and modification of the biological carrying capacity. A recent archaeological study [114] suggested that large cultural changes facilitate human expansion to new areas. Building on this idea, migration could introduce new information to a population, leading to range expansion, which could be another sense in which cultural changes could generate population growth. This raises a direction of causality question in interpreting the Palaeolithic revolution: did increased migration bring about cultural bursts, leading to increased carrying capacities and resulting growth across populations? Or did a carrying-capacity-modifying innovation occur in one population, which in turn brought about cultural changes that subsequently facilitated migration, expansion and population growth?

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

diffusion of core technologies via rare migration, creating an increase in cultural complexity, which was coordinated among multiple localities yet rare enough to maintain differences between them, as seen in figure 5 (t , 500). Our model proposes that, with time, cultural evolution could have affected population sizes and, as a result, the migration rates between them, leading to decreasing cultural differentiation between local populations (figure 5, t . 500). The prehistoric record in Eurasia is characterized by a similar pattern: an increase in rates of population interaction in the Upper Palaeolithic [1,2,77,78], driven by population growth or by behavioural change, and replacement of the multitude of techno-complexes near the Middle –Upper Palaeolithic transition by the Aurignacian [86,96,97,99]. Another conspicuous archaeological pattern is the sporadic transient appearance of ‘advanced’ behaviours, characteristic of the Upper Palaeolithic (Late Stone Age in Africa), well within the Middle Stone Age: these include evidence of abstract art such as engraved ochre pieces and incised ostrich eggs, personal ornamentation such as shell beads, and advanced bone and stone tools [2,62,103–111]. A possible explanation of the transient nature of these phenomena is that, as in our simulations, the populations in which they occurred were too small and disconnected from one another to stably maintain complex culture (see also [18]). Many large-scale cultural shifts have been attributed to external factors, such as environmental change and resource availability [23,112,113], or cognitive and genetic changes [21,25 –27]; in these examples, non-cultural changes facilitate

25.

26.

28.

29.

30. 31.

32.

33.

34.

35.

36.

37.

38.

39.

56.

57.

58.

59.

60.

61.

62.

63.

64.

65.

66.

67.

68.

to climatic fluctuations. PLoS ONE 11, e0153277. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0153277) Gaudzinski-Windheuser S, Niven L. 2009 Hominin subsistence patterns during the Middle and Late Paleolithic in northwestern Europe. In The evolution of hominin diets: integrating approaches to the study of Palaeolithic subsistence (eds JJ Hublin, MP Richards), pp. 99 –111. Berlin, Germany: Springer. Lloveras L, Maroto J, Soler J, Thomas R, MorenoGarcı´a M, Nadal J, Soler N. 2016 The role of small prey in human subsistence strategies from Early Upper Palaeolithic sites in Iberia: the rabbits from the Evolved Aurignacian level of Arbreda Cave. J. Quat. Sci. 31, 458– 471. (doi:10.1002/jqs.2869) Pitulko VV, Nikolsky PA, Girya EY, Basilyan AE, Tumskoy VE, Koulakov SA, Astakhov SN, Pavlova EY, Anisimov MA. 2004 The Yana RHS site: humans in the Arctic before the last glacial maximum. Science 303, 52– 56. (doi:10.1126/science.1085219) O’Connell JF, Allen J, Hawkes K. 2010 Pleistocene Sahul and the origins of seafaring. In The global origins and development of seafaring (eds A Anderson, KV Boyle), pp. 57 –68. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge. O’Connell JF, Allen J. 2012 The restaurant at the end of the universe: modelling the colonisation of Sahui. Aust. Archaeol. 74, 5–31. (doi:10.1080/03122417. 2012.11681932) Hoffecker JF. 2005 Innovation and technological knowledge in the Upper Paleolithic of northern Eurasia. Evol. Anthropol. Issues News Rev. 14, 186–198. (doi:10.1002/evan.20066) McBrearty S, Brooks AS. 2000 The revolution that wasn’t: a new interpretation of the origin of modern human behavior. J. Hum. Evol. 39, 453–563. (doi:10.1006/jhev.2000.0435) McDougall I, Brown FH, Fleagle JG. 2005 Stratigraphic placement and age of modern humans from Kibish, Ethiopia. Nature 433, 733 –736. (doi:10.1038/nature03258) White TD, Asfaw B, DeGusta D, Gilbert H, Richards GD, Suwa G, Howell FC. 2003 Pleistocene Homo sapiens from Middle Awash, Ethiopia. Nature 423, 742–747. (doi:10.1038/nature01669) Mellars P. 2005 The impossible coincidence. A single-species model for the origins of modern human behavior in Europe. Evol. Anthropol. 14, 12– 27. (doi:10.1002/evan.20037) O’Connell JF, Allen J. 2007 Pre-LGM Sahul (Pleistocene Australia-New Guinea) and the archaeology of early modern humans. In Rethinking the human revolution (eds P Mellars, K Boyle, O Bar-Yosef, C Stringer), pp. 395–410. McDonald Institute Monographs. Cambridge, UK: McDonald Institute. James HA, Petraglia M. 2005 Modern human origins and the evolution of behavior in the later Pleistocene record of South Asia. Curr. Anthropol. 46, S3 –S27. (doi:10.1086/444365) Ambrose SH. 1998 Chronology of the Later Stone Age and food production in East Africa. J. Archaeol. Sci. 25, 377–392. (doi:10.1006/jasc.1997.0277)

9

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

27.

40. Kempe M, Lycett SJ, Mesoudi A. 2014 From cultural traditions to cumulative culture: parameterizing the differences between human and nonhuman culture. J. Theor. Biol. 359, 29 –36. (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2014. 05.046) 41. Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Jarrick A, Wachtmeister C-A. 2008 Why does human culture increase exponentially? Theor. Popul. Biol. 74, 46 –55. (doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2008.04.007) 42. Fogarty L, Creanza N, Feldman MW. 2015 Cultural evolutionary perspectives on creativity and human innovation. Trends Ecol. Evol. 30, 736– 754. (doi:10. 1016/j.tree.2015.10.004) 43. Premo LS. 2012 Local extinctions, connectedness, and cultural evolution in structured populations. Adv. Complex Syst. 15, 1150002. (doi:10.1142/ S0219525911003268) 44. Baldini R. 2015 Revisiting the effect of population size on cumulative cultural evolution. J. Cogn. Cult. 15, 320– 336. (doi:10.1101/001529) 45. Henrich J, Boyd R, Derex M, Kline MA, Mesoudi A, Muthukrishna M, Powell AT, Shennan SJ, Thomas MG. 2016 Understanding cumulative cultural evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 201610005. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1610005113) 46. Strimling P, Sjo¨strand J, Enquist M, Eriksson K. 2009 Accumulation of independent cultural traits. Theor. Popul. Biol. 76, 77–83. (doi:10.1016/j.tpb.2009.04.006) 47. Moran PAP. 1950 Notes on continuous stochastic phenomena. Biometrika 37, 17 –23. (doi:10.1093/ biomet/37.1-2.17) 48. Enquist M, Ghirlanda S. 2007 Evolution of social learning does not explain the origin of human cumulative culture. J. Theor. Biol. 246, 129 –135. (doi:10.1016/j.jtbi.2006.12.022) 49. Mellars P, French JC. 2011 Tenfold population increase in Western Europe at the Neandertal-tomodern human transition. Science 333, 623 –628. (doi:10.1126/science.1206930) 50. Dolukhanov PM, Shukurov AM, Tarasov PE, Zaitseva GI. 2002 Colonization of Northern Eurasia by modern humans: radiocarbon chronology and environment. J. Archaeol. Sci. 29, 593– 606. (doi:10. 1006/jasc.2001.0753) 51. Bar-Yosef O, Belfer-Cohen A. 2013 Following Pleistocene road signs of human dispersals across Eurasia. Quat. Int. 285, 30– 43. (doi:10.1016/j. quaint.2011.07.043) 52. Churchill SE, Smith FH. 2000 Makers of the early Aurignacian of Europe. Am. J. Phys. Anthropol. 113, 61 –115. (doi:10.1002/1096-8644(2000)43:31þ ,61::AID-AJPA4.3.0.CO;2-3) 53. Churchill SE. 2001 Hand morphology, manipulation, and tool use in Neandertals and early modern humans of the Near East. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 98, 2953–2955. (doi:10.1073/pnas.061032198) 54. Richards MP, Trinkaus E. 2009 Isotopic evidence for the diets of European Neanderthals and early modern humans. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci USA 106, 16 034– 16 039. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0903821106) 55. El Zaatari S, Grine FE, Ungar PS, Hublin J-J. 2016 Neandertal versus modern human dietary responses

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

24.

innovation linked to rapid climate change. Nat. Commun. 4, 1905 –1909. (doi:10.1038/ ncomms2897) Dow GK, Reed CG. 2011 Stagnation and innovation before agriculture. J. Econ. Behav. Organ. 77, 339–350. (doi:10.1016/j.jebo.2010.11.006) Ambrose SH. 2010 Coevolution of composite-tool technology, constructive memory, and language. Curr. Anthropol. 51, S135–S147. (doi:10.1086/ 650296) Wadley L. 2010 Compound-adhesive manufacture as a behavioral proxy for complex cognition in the Middle Stone Age. Curr. Anthropol. 51, S111 –S119. (doi:10.1086/649836) DeBeaune SA. 2004 The invention of technology: prehistory and cognition. Curr. Anthropol. 45, 139–162. (doi:10.1086/381045) Kolodny O, Creanza N, Feldman MW. 2015 Evolution in leaps: the punctuated accumulation and loss of cultural innovations. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 112, E6762–E6769. (doi:10.1073/pnas.1520492112) Kolodny O, Creanza N, Feldman MW. 2016 Gamechanging innovations: how culture can change the parameters of its own evolution and induce abrupt cultural shifts. PLoS Comput. Biol. 12, e1005302. (doi:10.1371/journal.pcbi.1005302) Lee RB et al. 1968 Man the hunter. Piscataway, NJ: Transaction Publishers. Evenson RE, Gollin D. 2003 Assessing the impact of the green revolution, 1960 to 2000. Science 300, 758–762. (doi:10.1126/science.1078710) Derex M, Beugin M-P, Godelle B, Raymond M. 2013 Experimental evidence for the influence of group size on cultural complexity. Nature 503, 389–391. (doi:10.1038/nature12774) Collard M, Ruttle A, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ. 2013 Population size and cultural evolution in nonindustrial food-producing societies. PLoS ONE 8, 1– 6. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0072628) Mesoudi A. 2011 Variable cultural acquisition costs constrain cumulative cultural evolution. PLoS ONE 6, 15 –17. (doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0018239) Kobayashi Y, Aoki K. 2012 Innovativeness, population size and cumulative cultural evolution. Theor. Popul. Biol. 82, 38 –47. (doi:10.1016/j.tpb. 2012.04.001) Enquist M, Ghirlanda S, Eriksson K. 2011 Modelling the evolution and diversity of cumulative culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 412– 423. (doi:10.1098/ rstb.2010.0132) Lehmann L, Aoki K, Feldman MW. 2011 On the number of independent cultural traits carried by individuals and populations. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 366, 424–435. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2010.0313) Lewis HM, Laland KN. 2012 Transmission fidelity is the key to the build-up of cumulative culture. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 367, 2171– 2180. (doi:10.1098/rstb. 2012.0119) Shennan S. 2001 Demography and cultural innovation: a model and its implications for the emergence of modern human culture. Cambridge Archaeol. J. 11, 5– 16. (doi:10.1017/S0959774301000014)

96. Belfer-Cohen A, Goring-Morris AN. 2007 From the beginning: Levantine Upper Palaeolithic cultural change and continuity. In Rethinking the human revolution: new behavioural and biological perspectives on the origin and dispersal of modern humans (ed. P Mellars), pp. 199–205. Cambridge, UK: University of Cambridge. 97. Goring-Morris N, Davidzon A. 2006 Straight to the point: Upper Paleolithic Ahmarian lithic technology in the Levant. Anthropologie XLIV, 93 –111. 98. Mellars P. 1999 The Neanderthal problem continued. Curr. Anthropol. 40, 341–364. (doi:10. 1086/200024) 99. Teyssandier N, Bon F, Bordes J-G. 2016 Within projectile range: some thoughts on the appearance of the Aurignacian in Europe. J. Anthropol. Res. 66, 209–229. (doi:10.3998/jar. 0521004.0066.203) 100. Meignen L. 2012 Levantine perspectives on the Middle to Upper Paleolithic ‘transition’. Archaeol. Ethnol. Anthropol. Eurasia 40, 12 –21. (doi:10.1016/ j.aeae.2012.11.003) 101. Maı´llo-Ferna´ndez J-M. 2013 Thoughts for discussion with regards to the Middle-Upper Paleolithic transition in the Iberian Peninsula. In Pleistocene foragers on the Iberian Peninsula: their culture and environment (eds A Pastoors, B Auffermann), pp. 107–122. Mettmann, Germany: Wissenschaftliche Schriften des Neanderthal Museum. 102. Riel-Salvatore J, Clark GA. 2007 Asking new questions and using new methods to understand the Middle-Upper Paleolithic transition. Bar 1620, 1–10. 103. Henshilwood CS et al. 2002 Emergence of modern human behavior: Middle Stone Age engravings from South Africa. Science 295, 1278–1280. (doi:10. 1126/science.1067575) 104. Marean CW et al. 2007 Early human use of marine resources and pigment in South Africa during the Middle Pleistocene. Nature 449, 905–908. (doi:10. 1038/nature06204) 105. Jacobs Z, Roberts RG, Galbraith RF, Deacon HJ, Gru¨n R, Mackay A, Mitchell P, Vogelsang R, Wadley L. 2008 Ages for the Middle Stone Age of southern Africa: implications for human behavior and dispersal. Science 322, 733– 735. (doi:10.1126/ science.1162219) 106. Henderson Z. 2002 A dated cache of ostricheggshell flasks from Thomas’ Farm, Northern Cape Province, South Africa. South African Archaeol. Bull, 38– 40. (doi:10.2307/3889105) 107. Henshilwood C, d’Errico F, Vanhaeren M, Van Niekerk K, Jacobs Z. 2004 Middle stone age shell beads from South Africa. Science 304, 404. (doi:10. 1126/science.1095905) 108. Henshilwood CS, d’Errico F, Watts I. 2009 Engraved ochres from the middle stone age levels at Blombos Cave, South Africa. J. Hum. Evol. 57, 27 – 47. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2009.01.005) 109. d’Errico F, Henshilwood C, Vanhaeren M, Van Niekerk K. 2005 Nassarius kraussianus shell beads

10

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

84. D’Errico F, Zilha˜o J, Julien M, Baffier D, Pelegrin J. 1998 Neanderthal acculturation in Western Europe? Curr. Anthropol. 39, S1 –S44. (doi:10.1086/204689) 85. Zilha˜o J, d’Errico F, Bordes J-G, Lenoble A, Texier J-P, Rigaud J-P. 2006 Analysis of Aurignacian interstratification at the Chatelperronian-type site and implications for the behavioral modernity of Neandertals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 12 643 –12 648. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0605128103) 86. Zilha˜o J, d’Errico F. 2003 The chronology of the Aurignacian and Transitional technocomplexes. Where do we stand. In The Chronology of the Aurignacian and of the transitional technocomplexes: dating, stratigraphies, cultural implications: Proceedings of Symposium 6.1 of the XIVth Congress of the UISPP, (University of Lie`ge, Belgium, September 2–8, 2001) (eds J Zilha˜o, F D’Errico), pp. 313 –350. Lisbon, Portugal: Instituto Portugueˆs de Arqueologia. 87. Henrich J. 2001 Cultural transmission and the diffusion of innovations: adoption dynamics indicate that biased cultural transmission is the predominate force in behavioural change. Am. Anthropol. 103, 992 –1013. (doi:10.1525/aa.2001.103.4.992) 88. Rogers EM. 2010 Diffusion of innovations. New York, NY: Simon & Schuster. 89. Ford PD, Chan EY. 2003 Knowledge sharing in a multi-cultural setting: a case study. Knowl. Manag. Res. Pract. 1, 11– 27. (doi:10.1057/palgrave.kmrp. 8499999) 90. Bar-Yosef O. 2002 The chronology of the Middle Paleolithic of the Levant. In Neandertals and modern humans in western Asia (eds T Akazawa, K Aoki, O Bar-Yosef ), pp. 39– 56. Berlin, Germany: Springer. 91. Ronchitelli A, Benazzi S, Boscato P, Douka K, Moroni A. 2014 Comments on ‘Human-climate interaction during the Early Upper Paleolithic: Testing the hypothesis of an adaptive shift between the ProtoAurignacian and the Early Aurignacian’ by William E. Banks, Francesco d’Errico, Joa˜o Zilha˜o. J. Hum. Evol. 73, 107–111. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2013.12.010) 92. Zilha˜o J, d’Errico F. 1999 The chronology and taphonomy of the earliest Aurignacian and its implications for the understanding of Neandertal extinction. J. World Prehistory 13, 1–68. (doi:10. 1023/A:1022348410845) 93. Richter D, Tostevin G, Sˇkrdla P, Davies W. 2009 New radiometric ages for the Early Upper Palaeolithic type locality of Brno-Bohunice (Czech Republic): comparison of OSL, IRSL, TL and 14 C dating results. J. Archaeol. Sci. 36, 708–720. (doi:10.1016/j.jas. 2008.10.017) 94. Teyssandier N. 2006 Questioning the first Aurignacian: mono or multi cultural phenomenon during the formation of the Upper Paleolithic in Central Europe and the Balkans. Anthropologie 44, 9. 95. Benazzi S et al. 2011 Early dispersal of modern humans in Europe and implications for Neanderthal behaviour. Nature 479, 525–528. (doi:10.1038/ nature10617)

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

69. Klein RG. 2016 Issues in human evolution. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 113, 6345 –6347. (doi:10.1073/pnas. 1606588113) 70. Atkinson QD, Gray RD, Drummond AJ. 2008 mtDNA variation predicts population size in humans and reveals a major Southern Asian chapter in human prehistory. Mol. Biol. Evol. 25, 468 –474. (doi:10. 1093/molbev/msm277) 71. Mithen SJ. 1996 The prehistory of the mind: a search for the origins of art, religion, and science. London, UK: Thames & Hudson. 72. Klein RG. 2008 Out of Africa and the evolution of human behavior. Evol. Anthropol. 17, 267 –281. (doi:10.1002/evan.20181) 73. Klein RG, Bird DW. 2016 Shellfishing and human evolution. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 44, 198–205. (doi:10.1016/j.jaa.2016.07.008) 74. Sealy J. 2016 Cultural change, demography, and the archaeology of the last 100 kyr in southern Africa. In Africa from MIS 6-2: population dynamics and paleoenvironments (eds SC Jones, BA Stewart), pp. 65 –75. Berlin, Germany: Springer. 75. Collard M, Buchanan B, O’Brien MJ. 2013 Population size as an explanation for patterns in the Paleolithic archaeological record: more caution is needed. Curr. Anthropol. 54, S388 – S396. (doi:10.1086/673881) 76. Collard M, Vaesen K, Cosgrove R, Roebroeks W. 2016 The empirical case against the ‘demographic turn’ in Palaeolithic archaeology. Phil. Trans. R. Soc. B 371, 20150242. (doi:10.1098/rstb.2015.0242) 77. Duke C, Steele J. 2010 Geology and lithic procurement in Upper Palaeolithic Europe: a weights-of-evidence based GIS model of lithic resource potential. J. Archaeol. Sci. 37, 813 –824. (doi:10.1016/j.jas.2009.11.011) 78. Fe´blot-Augustins J. 1993 Mobility strategies in the late Middle Palaeolithic of central Europe and western Europe: elements of stability and variability. J. Anthropol. Archaeol. 12, 211–265. (doi:10.1006/jaar.1993.1007) 79. Bocquet-Appel J-P, Degioanni A. 2013 Neanderthal demographic estimates. Curr. Anthropol. 54, S202– S213. (doi:10.1086/673725) 80. Bocquet-Appel J-P, Demars P-Y, Noiret L, Dobrowsky D. 2005 Estimates of Upper Palaeolithic metapopulation size in Europe from archaeological data. J. Archaeol. Sci. 32, 1656– 1668. (doi:10.1016/j.jas. 2005.05.006) 81. Harris K, Nielsen R. 2016 The genetic cost of Neanderthal introgression. Genetics 203, 881–891. (doi:10.1534/genetics.116.186890) 82. d’Errico F et al. 2003 Archaeological evidence for the emergence of language, symbolism, and music –an alternative multidisciplinary perspective. J. World Prehistory 17, 1–70. (doi:10.1023/ A:1023980201043) 83. Zilha˜o J et al. 2010 Symbolic use of marine shells and mineral pigments by Iberian Neandertals. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 1023 –1028. (doi:10.1073/ pnas.0914088107)

111. Yellen JE. 1998 Barbed bone points: tradition and continuity in Saharan and sub-Saharan Africa. African Archaeol. Rev. 15, 173–198. (doi:10.1023/ A:1021659928822) 112. Mellars P. 2006 Why did modern human populations disperse from Africa ca. 60,000 years ago? A new model. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 103, 9381 –9387. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0510792103)

113. Mellars P. 2006 Going east: new genetic and archaeological perspectives on the modern human colonization of Eurasia. Science 313, 796 –800. (doi:10.1126/science.1128402) 114. Shea JJ, Sisk ML. 2010 Complex projectile technology and Homo sapiens dispersal into Western Eurasia. PaleoAnthropology 2010, 100–122. (doi:10.4207/PA.2010.ART36)

11

rsif.royalsocietypublishing.org

from Blombos Cave: evidence for symbolic behaviour in the Middle Stone Age. J. Hum. Evol. 48, 3–24. (doi:10.1016/j.jhevol.2004.09.002) 110. Texier P-J et al. 2010 A Howiesons Poort tradition of engraving ostrich eggshell containers dated to 60,000 years ago at Diepkloof Rock Shelter, South Africa. Proc. Natl Acad. Sci. USA 107, 6180 –6185. (doi:10.1073/pnas.0913047107)

J. R. Soc. Interface 14: 20170171

Greater than the sum of its parts? Modelling population contact and interaction of cultural repertoires.

Evidence for interactions between populations plays a prominent role in the reconstruction of historical and prehistoric human dynamics; these interac...
2MB Sizes 0 Downloads 9 Views