C International Psychogeriatric Association 2013 International Psychogeriatrics (2014), 26:2, 297–305  doi:10.1017/S1041610213001877

Frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology in elderly people: the moderating role of rumination ...........................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

Virginia Fernández-Fernández,1 María Márquez-González,2 Andrés Losada-Baltar1 and Rosa Romero-Moreno1 1 2

Psychology Department, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Alcorcón, Madrid, Spain Biological and Health Psychology Department, Universidad Autónoma de Madrid, Madrid, Spain

ABSTRACT

Background: The positive effects of leisure activities on depressive symptomatology are well known. However, the extent to which emotional regulation variables moderate that relationship has scarcely been studied, especially in older people. The aim of this study is to analyze the moderating role of rumination in the relation between leisure activities and depressive symptoms. Methods: Participants in this study were 311 people, aged 60 to 90 years (mean age: 71.27 years; SD: 6.99; 71.7% women). We evaluated depressive symptomatology, frequency of leisure activities, and rumination. We carried out a hierarchical regression analysis to confirm the moderating role of rumination. Results: We obtained a model that explains 39.4% of the variance of depressive symptomatology. Main effects were found for the frequency of leisure activities (β = −0.397; p < 0.01) and for rumination (β = 0.497; p < 0.01). Moreover, we found a significant effect of the interaction between frequency of leisure activities and rumination (β = 0.110; p < 0.05), suggesting that rumination plays a moderating role in the relation between leisure activities and depressive symptomatology. Conclusions: A risk profile of elderly people may consist of those who engage in low levels of leisure activities but also use more frequently the dysfunctional emotional regulation strategy of rumination. Key words: pleasant events, depression, rumination, aging, emotion regulation

Introduction Depression has been extensively studied in older adults. Although this population shows prevalence rates for depression somewhat lower than those of other age groups (Blazer, 2003), depression does constitute a significant public health issue (Fiske et al., 2009; Solhaug et al., 2012). More specifically, some studies reveal a prevalence rate of around 15% for clinically significant depressive disorders in older adults (Conner et al., 2010). As Blazer (2003) notes, depression can be considered the most common cause of emotional distress among elderly people, contributing to reduction in their quality of life. The study of the mechanisms involved in the origin and maintenance of depression in older adults, and in the development of intervention

Correspondence should be addressed to: Virginia Fernández-Fernández, Departamento de Psicología, Universidad Rey Juan Carlos, Avd. Atenas s/n 28922, Alcorcón, Madrid 28922, Spain. Phone: +34-914888912. Email: [email protected]. Received 16 Apr 2013; revision requested 20 May 2013; revised version received 17 Sep 2013; accepted 1 Oct 2013. First published online 19 November 2013.

strategies for reducing its prevalence, is of great scientific and social relevance. Several studies have provided empirical evidence on the association between depression and functional impairment, not only in population-based research but also in cross-sectional and follow-up studies in older adults (e.g. Brenes et al., 2005). The World Health Organization (2002) already considers this disorder as the main cause of disability among elderly people. Furthermore, other research has shown how, in the elderly people, depression is especially related to chronic medical illness, cognitive impairment, family disruption, and increased mortality (Alexopoulos et al., 2011). Relationship between frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology One of the main psychological models for explaining depression is that of Lewinsohn (1974), who considered this disorder as the result of a reduction of person’s activity levels, implying a reduction or loss of positive reinforcers. This model has

298

V. Fernández-Fernández et al.

received substantial empirical support in the general population (Manos et al., 2010). Focusing on older adults, the aging process is associated with several contextual (e.g. retirement) and internal (e.g. biological) changes in the person, which undoubtedly affect their patterns of activity and their access to reinforcers. For example, for elderly people, the loss of mobility may represent an important precedent of psychological distress, given that it involves a significant reduction in their social and leisure/free-time activities (Teychenne et al., 2008). Barcelos-Ferreira et al. (2013) found in a sample of community elderly persons significant and negative associations between depression diagnosis and physical activity and going to cinema. In turn, deficits in behavioral activation and lack of involvement in leisure activities may be considered both as associated factors involved in the development of depressive disorders in older adults and as components of depressive disorder, which facilitate its maintenance (Manos et al., 2010). For this reason, activity takes on special importance in old age, as highlighted in the main models of aging, whose hypotheses were developed with a focus on this issue. One of the most significant proposals in this area was the Activity Theory (Havighurst, 1963), according to which the social interaction associated with involvement in leisure activities significantly increases the quality of life of elderly people. For example, the study conducted by Dimidjian et al. (2006) pointed out the effectiveness of an intervention program based on behavioral activation rather than just a pharmacological and cognitive one (Dimidjian et al., 2006). Moreover, longitudinal studies have provided substantial empirical support for the benefits of leisure activities for both well-being and cognitive functioning in elderly people (Leung et al., 2010). Influence of rumination on the relationship between frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology Cognitive models for explaining depression (e.g. Beck et al., 1979) focus on cognitive vulnerability, which consists in the presence of dysfunctional cognitive schemata, activated in response to stressors related to specific deficits in the individual, and which lead to distortions in information processing (e.g. negative interpretation of ambiguous information, and focusing the attention on negative aspects). A range of studies have found significant relationships between the presence of cognitive distortions and higher levels of psychological maladjustment (Millan et al., 2012) in the general population. Furthermore, these cognitive distortions can contribute to

increasing and maintaining depressive mood, promoting depressive behaviors (inactivity or passivity, avoidance of situations, etc.). From a cognitive perspective, it is assumed that a change in the behavioral component in a patient of depression can be simply achieved through changing the dysfunctional cognitive schemata that are at the basis of such behavior (Beck et al., 1979). One of the mechanisms explored in this approach, as a potential factor involved in the duration of depression, is rumination. Rumination is defined as the tendency to focus attention on one’s own depressive symptoms and to have repetitive thoughts about the causes of such symptoms and/or the situation or event that generated them, as well as about the negative consequences of that situation or event and of the symptoms themselves (Abramson et al., 1989). Likewise, rumination has been also understood as an emotion regulation strategy displayed by the person as a response to the normal emotion of sadness (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). Rumination appears to have a key influence on the intensity and duration of depressive symptoms: People who present a ruminative style feel such symptoms more intensely and for longer periods than those who employ other strategies (e.g. distraction), in both general and elderly population groups (Aldao et al., 2010). Thus, the tendency to ruminate predicts the onset, duration, and severity of depression (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008), rumination also being one of the most common symptoms found in depressive states in elderly people, and one of the factors that triggers depressive episodes (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008). The harmful psychological effects of rumination have been analyzed in several studies, in longitudinal and experimental research in both the general population (Nolen-Hoeksema et al., 2008) and the dementia caregiving population (RomeroMoreno et al., 2011), as well as (although less frequently) in elderly people (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011). Specifically, the cross-sectional study carried out by Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao (2011) found that even when the frequency of rumination is lower in older adults than in younger ones, this strategy shows a negative correlation in both populations. In the general population, the ruminative style increases despair about the future as well as negative and global attributions through greater negativity based on the interpretation of negative events. Furthermore, it facilitates the emergence of thoughts about negative events and memories, reduces effectiveness in problem-solving, and increases vulnerability to the development of anxiety symptoms (NolenHoeksema et al., 2008).

Moderation role of rumination

A recent cross-sectional study with younger adults (Genet and Siemer, 2012) has found that rumination moderates the relationship between unpleasant daily events and negative mood in such a way that unpleasant daily events were significantly associated with higher levels of negative mood only in individuals with high levels of rumination and not in individuals with low levels of rumination. Taking into account these findings, it is theoretically possible that people’s tendency to ruminate might also moderate the intensity of the relationship between involvement in leisure activities and depressive symptomatology. Some of the studies mentioned in the above paragraphs may suggest the hypothesis that rumination plays a moderator role in this relationship insofar as it interferes with the emotional benefits derived from leisure activities. For example, Romero-Moreno et al. (in press) have recently pointed out how guilt (as a cognitive variable) has a moderating role between pleasant activities and depressive symptoms in caregivers of relatives with dementia (Romero-Moreno et al., in press). Hence, due to its persistent focus on negative emotions (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011), which may represent a substantial obstacle to obtaining gratification or a positive reward from pleasant activities, people with a tendency to ruminate might benefit less from their involvement in such pleasant activities. However, recent studies suggest that pleasant activities are subjectively associated with improved mood states and reduced ruminative thinking, reflecting an inverse relationship between involvement in pleasant activities and rumination (Takano et al., in press). Taking these findings into account, an alternative hypothesis on the specific moderator role of rumination may be that the beneficial effects of engagement in pleasant leisure activities are particularly important for people with a tendency to ruminate, as such involvement may help them to reduce the frequency of their negative thinking and depressive symptomatology. Identifying which variables moderate the impact of cognitive and behavioral therapy on depressive symptoms seems to be a useful strategy for increasing our knowledge about the profiles of older adults that benefit most from engagement in leisure activities and respond best to interventions (Dimidjian et al., 2011). Given the importance of participation in pleasant activities for older adults’ well-being and quality of life, and assuming the clinical relevance of depressive disorders in this population, we decided to explore previously described hypotheses in this age group. Therefore, this study is aimed at exploring the potential role of rumination as a moderator in the relationship

299

between participation in leisure activities and level of depressive symptomatology in older adults aged 60 years and over. In addition to testing the moderator role of rumination in this relationship, we were also interested in exploring which of the two specific hypotheses in the direction of moderation best fits the data: (a) the “rumination as interference” hypothesis, according to which the relationship between involvement in leisure activities and depression will be lower for older adults with high levels of rumination; or (b) the hypothesis that involvement in leisure activities will have a significant and stronger association with depressive symptomatology in older adults with a tendency to ruminate, as opposed to those with low levels of rumination.

Methods Participants Participants were 312 people aged over 60 years, non-institutionalized, and recruited at the Red Cross Reina Sofía Cultural Centre in Madrid, where they took advantage of the educational and cultural activities it offers. Measures In addition to the socio-demographic characteristics of the sample (age, sex, and educational level), we assessed the following variables. DEPRESSIVE SYMPTOMATOLOGY

For the assessment of depressive symptomatology we used the Center for Epidemiologic Studies Depression Scale (CES-D; Radloff, 1977), a 20item scale (e.g. “I felt sad”) that rates the frequency of appearance of depressive symptoms in the previous week by means of a four-option Likerttype response format (from 0 = rarely or none of the time, to 3 = most or all of the time). The cut-off point established in this scale to detect clinical depression is a score of 16. This scale has good psychometric properties, with an internal consistency index (Cronbach’s α) of 0.89 (Losada et al., 2012). In the present study, the α-value was 0.86. RUMINATION

This variable was measured with a brief version of the Response Styles Questionnaire, the Ruminative Responses Scale (RRS-Brief Version; Jackson and Nolen-Hoeksema, 1998), which assesses the frequency of appearance of ruminative thoughts in situations in which the person is experiencing feelings of sadness or melancholy. This scale

300

V. Fernández-Fernández et al.

consists of 10 items (e.g. “I think about how sad I feel”), with Likert-type response format and four options (from 0 = never, to 3 = always). This scale showed good psychometric properties in previous studies, with an internal consistency of 0.85 for general population and of 0.91 in elderly people (Márquez-González et al., 2012). In this work the α-value was 0.89. FREQUENCY OF LEISURE/FREE-TIME ACTIVITIES

This variable was assessed through the adaptation of the leisure time satisfaction (LTS) measure (Stevens et al., 2004). This instrument evaluates, through six items (e.g. “Eating out or other social activities”), how often the person performed different leisure activities in the previous month, with item scores ranging from 0 (not at all) to 4 (a lot). This scale showed acceptable psychometric properties in previous studies, with an internal consistency index of 0.73 (Losada et al., 2010). The α-value obtained here with this instrument was 0.73. Procedure Participants were contacted at a cultural center (Red Cross Centre Reina Sofía) in Madrid, where they were attending a range of courses (literature, drawing, computing, etc.). Posters and pamphlets provided information about the study and invited voluntary participation. Those who wished to participate were required, after providing written informed consent, to fill an assessment protocol, aimed at screening the mental health of the center’s users. A requirement for participation was basic literacy. Two specially trained psychologists were present during the assessments, and answered the participants’ questions about how to respond to the items. Time taken for the assessments was approximately 30–40 minutes. Data analysis We first carried out an analysis of the univariate or multivariate atypical cases. For the description of the sample we carried out descriptive and frequency analyses. With the aim of minimizing the effects of multi-collinearity, we carried out all the regression analyses with the standardized independent variables centered. Furthermore, we examined the variance inflation factor (VIF) and the tolerance indices. The relationship between the variables assessed was measured by means of bivariate Pearson’s correlations analyses. We carried out hierarchical regression analyses to determine, on the one hand, the main effects of the frequency of leisure activities and rumination variables on the dependent variable (depressive

symptomatology), and, on the other hand, the moderation effect of rumination on the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology. Following Baron and Kenny’s (1986) criteria, there is a moderation effect when the product of the predictor variable (frequency of leisure activities) by the moderator (rumination) is significant in the explanation of the criterion variable (depressive symptomatology) after controlling the main effects of the two variables. In the first step of the hierarchical regression analysis, sex was entered, the frequency of leisure activities being entered in the second step. The third step saw the entry of rumination, and finally, the interaction “frequency of leisure activities by rumination” was entered in the fourth step. Likewise, to determine the nature of the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology, we carried out a post hoc analysis following the procedure described by Holmbeck (2002), according to which the moderation effect is confirmed through the different levels of the moderator variable (high levels of rumination [+1 SD] vs. low levels [–1 SD]) in the relation between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology. Lastly, following the procedure described by Preacher et al. (2006), we calculated the specific values of the significant regions of rumination in which the regression of the frequency of leisure activities on depressive symptomatology moves from being non-significant to significant.

Results Description of the sample The analysis of univariate and multivariate atypical cases revealed the presence of a univariate atypical case, and no multivariate atypical cases were found. Thus, the final sample of 311 participants was obtained. Mean age of participants in the sample was 71.27 years (SD = 6.9; range 60–90 years), with 71.7% women. Mean of number of years of formal education was 10.96 (SD = 6.45). The moderation role of rumination between leisure activities and depressive symptomatology Table 1 shows score results for the variables studied. The correlation analyses carried out suggest that both frequency of leisure activities and rumination are significantly associated with depressive symptomatology (Table 1). Likewise, we found a significant negative relationship between the frequency of activities and rumination.

Moderation role of rumination

301

Table 1. Matrix correlation and descriptive data 1

2

3

MEAN

SD

RANGE OF SCORES

............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

1. Depressive symptomatology 2. Frequency of leisure activities 3. Rumination

– −0.16∗ 0.58∗

– – −0.22∗

– – –

16.86 11.23 10.18

7.94 5.47 6.20

0–60 0–30 0–24

Note: ∗ p ࣘ 0.01.

Table 2. Analysis of main effects and interaction effects β

t

ΔR 2

............................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................................

1.

2.

3.

4.

Sex

Sex Frequency of leisure activities Sex Frequency of leisure activities Rumination Sex Frequency of leisure activities Rumination Frequency of leisure activities x rumination

− 0.14∗ ∗∗

− 0.16 − 0.39∗∗ ∗

− 0.10 − 0.19∗∗ 0.49∗∗ ∗

− 0.09 − 0.19∗∗ 0.47∗∗ − 0.11∗

14.87 − 2.51 16.63 − 3.20 − 7.67 17.73 − 2.31 − 4.01 10.09 17.03 − 2.11 − 4.00 9.46 − 2.39

0.02∗ 0.16∗∗ 0.20∗∗

0.01∗

Note: ΔR2 : Increase in the percentage of explained variance; x: product; ∗∗ p < 0.01; ∗ p < 0.05; 0 = female; 1 = male.

The final regression model explains 39.3% of the variance of depressive symptomatology. The results of this analysis are shown in Table 2. Significant main effects on depressive symptomatology in the final model were found for sex, frequency of leisure activities, and rumination. Furthermore, there was a significant interaction effect of frequency of leisure activities and rumination. These results suggest that rumination has a moderating effect on the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology; in other words, the relationship between leisure activities and depressive symptomatology is moderated by the level of rumination in elderly people. The post hoc analyses carried out on the interactive effect of rumination on the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology (see Figure 1) reveal that this relationship varies in accordance with high levels (+1 SD) or low levels (−1 SD) of the moderator variable. The results show that when rumination was high, the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology was significant (β = −0.29; t = −4.55; p = 0.000). Specifically, when the frequency of leisure activities was low, the average for depressive symptomatology was 22.68, compared

with 18.05 when the frequency of leisure activities was high. However, when rumination was low, the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology was not significant (β = −0.08; t = −1.21; p = 0.22). The score range for depressive symptomatology was from an average of 13.26 when the frequency of leisure activities was low to 12.05 when it was high. The results obtained after calculation of the region of significance of rumination indicate that the frequency of leisure activities significantly predicts depressive symptomatology when elderly people obtain scores of 5.90 or more on the rumination scale.

Discussion The object of this study was to explore the potential moderator role of rumination in the relationship between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology in people aged over 60 years. Specifically, two alternative moderation hypotheses suggesting different directions of moderation were compared. The results of the study support the role of rumination as a moderating variable of the relationship between the frequency of leisure

302

V. Fernández-Fernández et al.

Figure 1. (Colour online) Estimation of the relation between the frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology in participants with high (+ 1 SD) and low (−1 SD) levels of rumination.

activities and depressive symptomatology among elderly people. Specifically, we found evidence to support the hypothesis that the relationship between involvement in leisure activities and depression is significant only for older adults with high levels of rumination (hypothesis (b)). The results suggest that the psychological correlates (depressive symptomatology) of leisure activities are especially important for older adults with a tendency to ruminate, as only in this group the frequency (high or low) of leisure activities is significantly associated with different emotional correlates: a higher frequency of leisure activities is associated with lower levels of depressive symptomatology, and scarce involvement in such activities is linked to higher levels of depression. Although low involvement in leisure activities is associated with higher levels of depressive symptomatology in all participants, low frequency of leisure activities is more strongly related to depressive symptomatology in elderly people who tend to use rumination as an emotional regulation strategy. It should be noted that the relationship between leisure activities and depression is not significant in the presence of low levels of rumination. This finding suggests that elderly people who perform fewer leisure activities and have a tendency to ruminate may represent a particularly vulnerable group with higher risk of depressive symptoms. As regards psychological intervention in elderly people with depressive symptomatology, our findings suggest the importance of considering behavioral (leisure activities) and cognitive variables (rumination) jointly in order to explain, prevent,

and/or treat depressive symptomatology in older adults. The results of the study support a significant inverse relationship between the performance of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology in elderly people as previously found in research with the general population (Lewinsohn, 1974; Manos et al., 2010). This finding highlights the importance of the involvement of older adults in leisure activities for their emotional wellbeing. In line with the findings of Sütterlin et al. (2012) in elderly people, our results also support the association between rumination and depressive symptomatology, which demonstrates the relevance of this emotional regulation variable in the explanation of depressive symptomatology for this population (Nolen-Hoeksema and Aldao, 2011). Even though both leisure activities and rumination have been considered in previous studies as involved in the development and maintenance of depressive disorders in older adults, to our knowledge this is the first study to show a moderator role for rumination in the relationship between leisure activities and depression. As our results suggest, rumination is not only a variable associated with distress in elderly people but may also reinforce the detrimental effect of scarce involvement in leisure activities in this population. The present results are of great relevance for the understanding of depressive symptomatology in older people, as they highlight the importance of taking into consideration both behavioral (involvement in leisure activities) and cognitive

Moderation role of rumination

(rumination) variables in order to gain a deeper understanding of the depression phenomenon in elderly people. As shown by our data regarding the percentage of variance in depressive symptomatology, both factors seem to be relevant in explaining this variable (15.7% involvement in leisure activities and 20.5% in rumination). These results must be considered with caution, given the correlational nature of this study, which precludes us from drawing conclusions about possible causal relations between rumination, frequency of leisure activities, and depressive symptomatology. Thus, there is a need for longitudinal and experimental studies that will allow us to test hypotheses of a causal nature. The study has some additional limitations. First, the participants were recruited at a cultural center, which undoubtedly affects the representativeness of the sample. Moreover, despite presenting adequate psychometric properties, the scale employed in this study for assessing leisure activities does not cover the broad spectrum of gratifying activities; nor was it designed specifically for older adults. Also, important variables, such as marital status or educational level, have not been controlled in this study, even though they may play a significant role in the explanation of depression in older adults. Future studies should include these and other relevant variables in order to control their potential influence for the variables of interest. In spite of the above, the finding that the relationship between the level of involvement in leisure activities and depressive symptomatology is influenced by a key cognitive–emotional variable, such as rumination, has clear implications for clinical practice. First of all, it seems reasonable to think that rumination should be considered in the clinical assessment of elderly people as a potential factor of psychological vulnerability, as previous studies have suggested (Genet and Siemer, 2012; Takano et al., in press). Specifically, the results suggest that such vulnerability may be especially significant in those cases where the scores on rumination are above 5.9, the specific cut-off point for a rumination score after which the frequency of leisure activities is significantly related to depressive symptomatology. These results are more relevant in older people, considering that rumination is regarded as one of the most damaging elements in adaptation to emotional losses (Nolen-Hoeksema and Davis, 2005), which are highly frequent as part of the aging process. From the prevailing paradigm of active aging, originally proposed by Rowe and Khan (1987), numerous policies have been launched aimed at the

303

promotion of well-being and quality of life in elderly people by encouraging participation in pleasant social activities (World Health Organization, 2002). The results of this study highlight the importance of analyzing psychological variables, including rumination, in the process and design of such active aging interventions. Specifically, our results suggest that this type of intervention may be particularly helpful and necessary for older adults with high levels of rumination. The results also highlight the need to develop effective psychological interventions in order to reduce rumination in older adults, since it is a factor that, in contrast to pleasurable activities, has been scarcely taken into account in assessments and clinical intervention with this population. The results presented here reveal the need to continue exploring the relationships and interactions between the diverse cognitive, emotional, and behavioral processes involved in depressive symptomatology, with the aim of bringing together different levels of analysis and gaining a more integrated and comprehensive understanding of the development and duration of depression in this age group.

Conflict of interests None.

Description of authors’ roles All the authors collaborated in the design and implementation of the study, the data analysis, and the writing of the paper.

Acknowledgments This work would not have been possible without the cooperation of the Cruz Roja Española, Centro de Mayores Reina Sofía (Spanish Red Cross, Reina Sofia Day Centre for Senior Citizens). The study was funded within the framework of Project PSI2009-08132 from the Ministry of Science and Innovation, and Project PSI201231293 from the Ministry of Economic and Competitiveness.

References Abramson, L. Y., Metalsky, F. I. and Alloy, L. B. (1989). Hopelessness depression: a theory based subtype of depression. Psychological Review, 96, 358–372. doi:10.1037/0033-295X.96.2.358.

304

V. Fernández-Fernández et al.

Aldao, A., Nolen-Hoeksema, S. and Schweizer, S. (2010). Regulation strategies across psychopathology: a meta-analytic review. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 217–237. doi:10.1016/j.cpr.2009.11.004. Alexopoulos, G. S. et al. (2011). Problem-solving therapy and supportive therapy in older adults with major depression and executive dysfunction: effect on disability. Archives of General Psychiatry, 68, 33. Barcelos-Ferreira, R., Yoshio Nakano, E., Steffens, D. C. and Bottino, C. (2013). Quality of life and physical activity associated to lower prevalence of depression in community-dwelling elderly subjects from Sao Paulo. Journal of Affective Disorders, 150, 616–622. Baron, R. M. and Kenny, D. A. (1986). The moderator-mediator variable distinction in social psychological research: conceptual, strategic and statistical considerations. Journal of Personality and Social Psychology, 51, 1173–1182. Beck, A. T., Rush, A. J., Shaw, B. F. and Emery, G. (1979). Cognitive Therapy for Depression. New York, NY: Guilford Press. Blazer, D. G. (2003). Depression in late life: review and commentary. Journal of Gerontology: Medical Sciences, 58A, 249–265. Brenes, G. A. et al. (2005). The influence of anxiety on the progression of disability. Journal of the American Geriatrics Society, 53, 34–39. doi:10.1111/j.1532-5415.2005. 53007.x. Conner, K. O. et al. (2010). Mental health treatment seeking among older adults with depression: the impact of stigma and race. American Journal of Geriatric Psychiatry, 18, 531. Dimidjian, S. et al. (2006). Randomized trial of behavioral activation, cognitive therapy, and antidepressant medication in the acute treatment of adults with major depression. Journal of Consulting and Clinical Psychology, 74, 658. Dimidjian, S., Barrera, M., Martell, C., Muñoz, R. F. and Lewinsohn, P. M. (2011). The origins and current status of behavioral activation treatments for depression. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 7, 1–38. doi:10.1146/annurev-clinpsy-032210-104535. Fiske, A., Wetherell, J. L. and Gatz, M. (2009). Depression in older adults. Annual Review of Clinical Psychology, 5, 363–389. doi:10.1146/annurev.clinpsy.032408.153621. Genet, J. J. and Siemer, M. (2012). Rumination moderates the effects of daily events on negative mood: results from a diary study. Emotion, 12, 1329–39. doi:10.1037/a0028070. Havighurst, R. J. (1963). Successful aging. In R. H. Williams, C. Tibbitts and W. Donohue (eds.), Processes of Aging: Social and Psychological Perspectives (pp. 299–320). New York, NY: Atherton. Holmbeck, G. N. (2002). Post-hoc probing of significant moderational and mediational effects in studies of pediatric populations. Journal of Pediatric Psychology, 27, 87–96. Jackson, B. and Nolen-Hoeksema, S. (1998). Mediators of the gender difference in rumination. Psychology of Women Quarterly, 25, 37–47. doi:10.1111/1471-6402.00005. Leung, G. T. et al. (2010). Examining the association between participation in late-life leisure activities and cognitive function in community-dwelling elderly Chinese

in Hong Kong. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 2–13. doi:10.1017/S1041610209991025. Lewinsohn, P. M. (1974). The behavioral study and treatment of depression. In K. S. Calhoun, H. E. Adams and K. M. Mitchell (eds.), Innovative Treatment Methods in Psychopathology (pp. 63–123). New York, NY: Wiley. Losada, A., Márquez-González, M., Peñacoba, C. and Romero-Moreno, R. (2010). Development and validation of the Caregiver Guilt Questionnaire. International Psychogeriatrics, 22, 650–660. doi:10.1017/S1041610210000074. Losada, A., Márquez-González, M., García-Ortíz, L., Gómez-Marcos, M. A., Fernández-Fernández, V. and Rodríguez-Sánchez, E. (2012). Loneliness and mental health in a representative sample of community-dwelling Spanish older adults. Journal of Psychology: Interdisciplinary and Applied, 146, 277–292. doi:10.1080/00223980.2011.582523. Manos, R. C., Kanter, J. W. and Busch, A. M. (2010). A critical review of assessment strategies to measure the behavioral activation model of depression. Clinical Psychology Review, 30, 547–561. doi:10.1016/j. cpr.2010.03.008. Márquez-González, M., Losada, A., Fernández-Fernández, V. and Pachana, N. (2012). Psychometric properties of the Spanish versión of the Geriatric Anxiety Inventory. International Psychogeriatrics, 24, 137–144. doi:10.1017/S1041610211001505. Millan, M. J. et al. (2012). Cognitive dysfunction in psychiatric disorders: characteristics, causes and the quest for improved therapy. Nature Reviews Drug Discovery, 11, 141–168. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. and Aldao, A. (2011). Gender and age differences in emotion regulation strategies and their relationship to depressive symptoms. Personality and Individual Differences, 51, 704–770. doi:10.1016/j.paid.2011.06.012. Nolen-Hoeksema, S. and Davis, C. G. (2005). Positive responses to loss: perceiving benefits and growth. In C. R. Snyder and S. J. López (eds.), Handbook of Positive Psychology (pp. 598–603). Oxford, UK: Oxford University Press. Nolen-Hoeksema, S., Wisco, B. and Lyubomirsky, S. (2008). Rethinking rumination. Perspectives on Psychological Science, 3, 400–424. doi:10.1111/j.1745-6924.2008.00088.x. Preacher, K. J., Curran, P. J. and Bauer, D. J. (2006). Computational tools for probing interactions in multiple linear regression, multilevel modeling, and latent curve analysis. Journal of Educational Behavioral Statistics, 31, 437–448. doi:10.3102/10769986031004437. Radloff, L. S. (1977). The CES-D Scale: a self-report depression scale for research in the general population. Applied Psychological Measurement, 1, 385–401. doi:10.1177/014662167700100306. Romero-Moreno, R., Márquez-González, M., Losada, A. and López, J. (2011). Motives for caring: relationship to stress and coping dimensions. International Psychogeriatrics, 23, 573–582. Romero-Moreno, R. et al. (in press). Leisure, gender, and kinship in dementia caregiving: psychological

Moderation role of rumination vulnerability of caregiving daughters with feelings of guilt. Journal of Gerontology B: Psychological Sciences and Social Sciences. doi:10.1093/geronb/ gbt027. Rowe, J. W. and Khan, R. L. (1987). Human aging: usual and successful. Science, 237, 143–149. doi:10.1126/science.3299702. Solhaug, H. I., Romuld, E. B., Romild, U. and Stordal, E. (2012). Increased prevalence of depression in cohorts of the elderly: an 11-year follow-up in the general population – the HUNT study. International Psychogeriatrics, 24, 151–158. doi:10.1017/S1041610211001141.

305

Stevens, A. B. et al. (2004). Measurement of leisure time satisfaction in family caregivers. Aging and Mental Health, 8, 450–459. doi:10.1080/13607860410001709737. Takano, K., Sakamoto, S. and Tanno, Y. (in press). Ruminative self-focus in daily life: associations with daily activities and depressive symptoms. Teychenne, M., Ball, K. and Salmon, J. (2008). Physical activity and likelihood of depression in adults: a review. Preventive Medicine, 46, 397–411. doi:10.1016/ j.ypmed.2008.01.009. World Health Organization. (2002). Active Ageing. Geneva, Switzerland: WHO.

Frequency of leisure activities and depressive symptomatology in elderly people: the moderating role of rumination.

The positive effects of leisure activities on depressive symptomatology are well known. However, the extent to which emotional regulation variables mo...
193KB Sizes 0 Downloads 0 Views