Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481

Contents lists available at ScienceDirect

Behavioural Brain Research journal homepage: www.elsevier.com/locate/bbr

Research report

Facilitated acquisition of standard but not long delay classical eyeblink conditioning in behaviorally inhibited adolescents M.D. Caulfield a,b , K.M. VanMeenen b , R.J. Servatius a,b,∗ a b

Stress and Motivated Behavior Institute, USA Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences, New Jersey Medical School, Rutgers Biomedical and Health Sciences, Newark, NJ, USA

h i g h l i g h t s • • • •

Inhibited individuals demonstrate facilitated learning at standard delay (500-ms) eyeblink conditioning. There are no differences in learning between inhibited and non-inhibited individuals at long delay (1000-ms) conditioning. Facilitated learning of standard delay eyeblink conditioning supports a cerebellar role in anxiety vulnerability. Adolescents demonstrate similar learning in delay and long delay eyeblink conditioning to young adults.

a r t i c l e

i n f o

Article history: Received 26 March 2014 Received in revised form 10 October 2014 Accepted 21 October 2014 Available online 30 October 2014 Keywords: Anxiety vulnerability Behavioral inhibition Delay eyeblink conditioning Long delay eyeblink conditioning Adolescent anxiety

a b s t r a c t Adolescence is a key age in the development of anxiety disorders. The present study assessed the relationship between behavioral inhibition, a risk factor for anxiety typified by avoidance, and acquisition of the classically conditioned eyeblink response. 168 healthy high school students (mean age 15.7 years, 54% female) were given a battery of self-report measures including the Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (AMBI). The study compared acquisition of three experimental training conditions. Two groups were given paired CS–US training: standard delay of 500-ms or long delay of 1000-ms with CS overlapping and co-terminating with a 50-ms airpuff US. A third group received unpaired training of 1000-ms CS and 50-ms airpuff US. Inhibited individuals showed greater acquisition of the conditioned eyeblink response in the 500-ms CS condition, but not in the paired 1000-ms condition. No differences in spontaneous blinks or reactivity to the stimulus were evident in the 1000-ms unpaired CS condition. Results support a relationship between associative learning and anxiety vulnerability that may be mediated by cerebellar functioning in inhibited individuals. © 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

1. Introduction Adolescence is a key period for the development of anxiety disorders. With a median age of 11, onset of anxiety occurs much earlier than any other psychiatric illness. Furthermore, half of all lifelong cases of clinical anxiety begin by age 14 [1,2]. The sensitive period of adolescence provides a unique opportunity to study the development of anxiety disorders. So far, it appears that a combination of vulnerabilities contribute to increased risk for developing clinical anxiety. In this vein, a stress-diathesis model emphasizes that the convergence of such factors including genetics, biology, sex, prior experience and personality alters reactivity to stressors in

∗ Corresponding author E-mail address: [email protected] (R.J. Servatius). http://dx.doi.org/10.1016/j.bbr.2014.10.027 0166-4328/© 2014 Elsevier B.V. All rights reserved.

the environment [3]. Recent research suggests that individual differences in learning may also be an important risk factor for anxiety vulnerability [4–6]. Behavioral inhibition (BI) is a personality factor linked to the development of anxiety disorders [7–9]. BI is observable early in life and persists through the lifespan [10]. Individuals with BI demonstrate similar physiological and behavioral profiles as those with clinical anxiety including altered heart rate reactivity [11,12], adrenocortical activity [13], apprehension, withdrawal and avoidance [14,15]. Avoidance is a key symptom of both behavioral inhibition and clinical anxiety [12,16,17], suggesting it is an essential component in the development and maintenance of anxiety. Avoidance is a learned response that is acquired and reinforced over time. As such, avoidance can be measured by assessing acquisition of negative reinforcement contingencies. Those vulnerable to anxiety

M.D. Caulfield et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481

disorders may be more susceptible to acquire and repeatedly express avoidant behaviors, leading to the avoidant thoughts and behaviors associated with clinical anxiety. It is still unclear which factors underlying avoidance acquisition are essential in the development of anxiety. One possibility is that anxious individuals are more sensitive to the cues and contingencies in their environments, resulting in faster learning and better performance on avoidance tasks. This theory is supported by the observation of individual differences in learning in both operant and classical conditioning avoidance paradigms [4–6,18,19]. Although operant paradigms may seem more suited to the high cognitive processes typically associated with avoidance, eyeblink classical conditioning is an established and reliable model for understanding human learning. In classical eyeblink conditioning, a conditioned stimulus (CS) and unconditional stimulus (US) are repeatedly paired, resulting in the acquisition of a conditioned response (CR), the measure of learning. Eyeblink classical conditioning is also one of the few preparations that has an advanced understanding of the neural substrates underlying acquisition with general consensus that the cerebellum is both necessary and sufficient to acquire standard delay eyeblink conditioning [20–23]. Variations of this basic paradigm have shown that rates of acquisition are affected by prior experience with the CS and US (e.g., proactive interference; [4]), by altering the reinforcement schedule [6], or by adjusting the contingencies between the CS and US such as in long delay and trace paradigms [24]. The effects of development on the acquisition of the conditioned eyeblink response have been assessed at length in infants and adults, largely overlooking the period of adolescence. Eyeblink conditioning has been used to demarcate the development of key underlying neural substrates in infants and young children [25–28]. Research in adults concentrates on aging to understand the neurobiology underlying age-related memory disorders [29–32]. Considering that adolescence is a critical period in refining cortical connections as well as for the development of psychopathologies such as anxiety and schizophrenia [33–35] understanding how eyeblink conditioning is affected may shed important light on underlying neural networks. Using eyeblink conditioning, we found that college-aged participants who score high on the Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (AMBI), a self-report measure of behaviorally inhibited temperament, demonstrated significantly faster learning in a standard delay (500-ms) conditioning paradigm[5]. At face value, this indicates that there is something fundamentally different about how behaviorally inhibited individuals learn about the basic stimuli in their environments, regardless of valence. However, the underlying processes are still unknown. The purpose of the present study was twofold: First, we utilized a basic science approach to assess acquisition of standard delay eyeblink in an adolescent sample for comparisons to other age groups. Second, we addressed two possible theories underlying facilitated learning observed in anxiety vulnerable individuals by comparing acquisition of standard delay (500-ms) to long delay (1000-ms) CS durations. Longer CS durations have slower ontogenetic development [27] and can be more difficult to acquire than standard delay durations [24,27,36]. Additionally, long-delay and trace paradigms demonstrate similar learning curves, with a reduction of learning in long delay, and a drastic reduction in trace paradigms following hippocampal lesion in rats [24,36]. Although delay conditioning is typically considered as cerebellar and trace conditioning as hippocampal, the dichotomy between the two paradigms is not so clear-cut. Evidence suggests the hippocampus is involved during delay eyeblink conditioning. Pyramidal neurons in the hippocampus show increased responding during the CS period in conjunction with development of

477

the behavioral CR, declining with continued training [37]. Neuroimaging studies reflect a similar pattern of activity in the hippocampus during delay eyeblink acquisition [38,39], suggesting that although the hippocampus is not essential in standard delay eyeblink acquisition it still plays a role under normal learning circumstances. Although the hippocampus is involved during normal learning, hippocampal lesion typically does not affect delay eyeblink conditioning and can actually enhance learning, suggesting the hippocampus may interfere with cerebellar functioning [40]. This is supported by studies assessing acquisition of eyeblink conditioning following hippocampal lesion in delay, long delay, and trace paradigms. Beylin et al. [24] found that unlesioned rats took longer to acquire long delay eyeblink conditioning than standard delay, with acquisition rates similar to that observed in trace conditioning. Additionally, acquisition of long delay eyeblink conditioning was significantly slower in the hippocampal lesioned rats, suggesting that the hippocampus plays a role in its acquisition. Developmental work supports this finding, demonstrating that infant rats can acquire short-delay conditioning, but are impaired at acquiring both long-delay and trace conditioning, which emerges in parallel later in development [41,42]. Therefore, long CS durations provide a useful paradigm to explore the influence of the hippocampus on acquisition without altering the conditioning parameters as drastically as trace conditioning would. Comparing acquisition of eyeblink conditioning in long and short delays provides a means for understanding whether the neural basis for enhanced acquisition in BI is primarily through reduced hippocampal involvement. Reduced hippocampal involvement in BI would be evident as faster short delay (as previously observed; [5]), concomitant with impaired long delay. If BI is associated with faster acquisition at both short and long delay, this finding suggests that either the facilitation is primarily mediated through the essential cerebellum brainstem/cerebellar circuitry or through those modulatory sites whose influence are in the same direction (accentuation of excitatory influence or diminution of inhibitory influence). 2. Materials and methods 2.1. Participants 168 participants were recruited from a local public high school in New Jersey. Participant’s ages ranged from 13 to 19 (M = 15.7, SD = 1.25). Parental consent forms were signed prior to participation for all students, as well as informed assent (participants under 18) or informed consent (18 and over) in accordance with procedures approved by the high school and University of Medicine and Dentistry of New Jersey Institutional Review Board. 2.2. Self report measures Participants completed self-report measures including the Adult and Retrospective Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (AMBI/RMBI: [43]), and the State/Trait Anxiety Inventory (STAI; [44]). The Adult and Retrospective Measure of Behavioural Inhibition (AMBI/RMBI; [43]) is a self-report measure that assesses inhibition or avoidance in response to new stimuli or social situations. It is reliable and has high discriminant validity in separating anxiety, depression, and control groups [43]. Scores on the 16-item AMBI range from 0 to 32 and include questions about current behaviors such as “Do you tend to withdraw and retreat from those around you?”, and “Do you tend to introduce yourself to new people?”. Scores on the 18-item RMBI range from 0 to 36 and include questions about childhood (during elementary school) behavior.

478

M.D. Caulfield et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481

The Spielberger State/Trait Anxiety Inventory is a 40-item selfreport questionnaire with total scores ranging from 40 to 160. The STAI is separated into two parts, State and Trait anxiety, each consisting of 20 questions: State Anxiety is assumed to change with mood and emotion and asks questions about the current emotional state of the participant such as “I am tense” and “I feel at ease”. Trait Anxiety is a relatively stable personality characteristic and asks questions about general feelings and behaviors such as “I feel nervous and restless” and “I feel satisfied with myself” [44].

Table 1 Number of participants and mean score on the Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition in each condition. Condition

500-ms paired 1000-ms paired 1000-ms unpaired

Inhibited High AMBI (>15.5)

Non-inhibited Low AMBI ( .102) nor was the distribution of male and female participants, X2 (1, N = 123) = .659, p = .417. Given previous research with these measures in eyeblink conditioning [5], we chose to concentrate primarily on AMBI scores to establish comparison groups. Participants were classified into inhibited and non-inhibited groups by using the median of the sample (15.5) for each measure. Mean scores and demographic information for the three conditions are listed on Table 1. 3.2. Eyeblink We used a step-wise approach in this analysis. First, to see if there are any differences of acquisition in the paired CS–US learning condition we used a 2 (Group: High/Low AMBI) × 2 (Condition: 500/1000) × 6 (Block) mixed measures ANOVA. A Group × Block interaction, F(5,480) = 5.039, p < .01, indicates differences in overall learning between groups. Post-hoc repeated measures ANVOAs revealed that significant learning occurred in the 500-ms condition, F(5,240) = 13.424, p < .01, but not in the 1000-ms condition, F(5,240) = 0.671, p = .646, see Fig. 1. There was also a significant

2.5. Signal processing Eyelid EMG recordings were evaluated for each participant on a trial-by-trial basis. To be recorded as an eyeblink response the smoothed signal must change by more than the mean activity plus 4 times the standard deviation in a 125-ms comparator window occurring immediately before the onset of the CS [45]. To maintain continuity between conditions, responses were only counted as a CR if it occurred within the 500-ms prior to the onset of the US. Eyeblink sessions with excessive signal noise (loss of more than 10% of trials), incomplete session data (e.g., falling asleep), or that demonstrated a lack of a UR were discarded and not used from further

Fig. 1. Acquisition of the eyeblink CR for groups receiving a 500-ms or 1000-ms paired CS duration. The acquisition session consisted of 6 blocks of 10 paired CS–US trials. Significant acquisition of the CR was present in the 500-ms CS duration, but not the 1000-ms CS duration, as evidenced by a Group × Block interaction. Follow-up post-hoc tests revealed that significant learning over the acquisition period occurred in the 500-ms condition, but not the 1000-ms condition. Error bars represent the standard error of the mean.

M.D. Caulfield et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481

Fig. 2. Follow-up independent samples t-tests exploring the significant Group × Condition interaction indicated that high AMBI had more overall CR responses than low AMBI. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

between-subjects Group × Condition interaction, F(1,96) = 4.788, p = .031. Follow-up independent samples t-tests indicated that high AMBI had more overall CR responses than low AMBI at 500ms, t(48) = −2.897, p = 0.006, but not at 1000-ms, t(48) = −0.079, p = .937, see Fig. 2. There was no difference of initial UR amplitudes between the conditions, F(1,99) = 1.388, p = .242, or between the AMBI groups, F(1,99) = 636, p = .427. Analysis of the other self-report measures used (RMBI, State, and Trait) revealed no significant learning differences, all p’s > 0.068. Finally, there we no significant sex differences in acquisition, all p’s > .0328. Longer CS durations are prone to capture a greater number of non-specific blinks. To assess the prevalence of non-specific blinks at 1000-ms we compared the CS–US paired condition to the explicitly unpaired condition that presented the CS and US stimuli separately. Using the CS alone trials from the 1000-ms

479

unpaired condition, A 2 (Group: High/Low) × 2 (Condition: Paired/Unpaired) × 6 (Block) mixed measures ANOVA revealed a between-subjects effect of condition, F(1,69) = 17.392, p .091. Finally, assessment of the US alone trials in the unpaired condition demonstrated no differences between responses made to the CS alone to those made prior to the onset of the US, F(5,220) = .613, p = .690, suggesting that the CR rate for the unpaired condition is simply a measure of spontaneous blinking during eyeblink classical conditioning. 4. Discussion Considering the transition to clinical anxiety during adolescence and early adulthood, it follows that understanding anxiety vulnerability at this key period is essential. Here, we extend previous research in adults to demonstrate that that eyeblink conditioning is also useful in differentiating vulnerability to anxiety disorders in adolescents. Further, acquisition of the conditioned eyeblink response was assessed under different CS durations. Overall, learning was better at the 500-ms CS duration. At this duration, learning was not much different than has been previously reported in samples of college-aged adults [4–6]. Learning was significantly reduced in the 1000-ms paired CS condition, similar to what is observed in Herbert et al. [27] in adult long delay with a CS duration of 1250-ms. These findings are consistent with previous research demonstrating acquisition under long delay is more

Fig. 3. Analysis of the first 10 trials for each condition revealed a main effect of trial and a main effect of condition. Comparison of mean responses to the conditioned stimulus revealed that paired learning produced more CRs than unpaired conditioning. Error bars represent standard error of the mean.

480

M.D. Caulfield et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481

similar to trace than standard delay procedures in infant rats [42] and those with hippocampal lesions [24]. Although it should be noted that there have been some reports of the opposite effect, suggesting similarities between long-delay and standard delay conditioning using young adult rabbits [46,47]. It is clear that continued research using long-delay methods is necessary to fully understand the essential underlying neural substrates and behavioral outcomes specifically in humans. Previously, we demonstrated that those who score high on AMBI acquire standard delay eyeblink conditioning to a greater extent [5]. This initial finding demonstrated an important relationship between self-report measures of anxiety vulnerability and associative learning, suggesting that there is something fundamentally different about how inhibited individuals’ process the stimuli in their environment. Here, we compared acquisition of standard delay to a longer CS duration. Differences in learning at these two CS durations shed light on underlying neural substrates influencing acquisition. Hippocampal dysfunction in inhibited individuals is one viable explanation for previously observed learning differences in standard eyeblink conditioning. Previous research indicates the hippocampus may be involved in risk for anxiety in terms of structure [48], function [49], and behavior [19]. Reduced hippocampal activity in the high AMBI group could enhance learning in standard delay (500-ms) conditioning. Alternatively, cerebellar activity could be altered. Recent neuroimaging studies indicate increased cerebellar activity in Lobule VI of inhibited individuals to novel faces [50], as well as during eyeblink conditioning [38]. It is possible that individual differences in activity in the Lobule VI region are contributing to facilitated eyeblink conditioning in high AMBI. Here, we found that those with high AMBI scores had more CRs at 500-ms, supporting previous research in college-age samples and extending these individual differences to adolescence. No differences in learning were observed at the 1000-ms paired condition, suggesting hippocampal dysfunction is not mediating observed individual differences. The Adult Measure of Behavioural Inhibition [43] is a useful tool in differentiating classical eyeblink conditioning. Here, we demonstrate that it is sensitive to associative learning differences in an adolescent sample, similar to previous observations in college-age students. The AMBI has not been used previously with an adolescent sample, therefore it is unclear precisely how valid the scores are. Comparison to unpublished data from the same population indicates that scores in this study were similar to those previously observed, t(461) = −2.887, p = .212. However, continued research assessing the validity of the AMBI measure in general, and specifically in adolescent populations is warranted. Future research in this area will contribute to understanding the role of the cerebellum in anxiety vulnerability. In recent years interest has increased in understanding the higher cognitive capacities of the cerebellum (for a review see [51]). Tract-tracing studies using transneuronal retrograde viruses indicate reciprocal connections between the non-motor cortical regions such as the dorsolateral prefrontal cortex and the cerebellar dentate nucleus [52,53]. Using resting state functional connectivity, researchers have demonstrated connections between non-motor cortical regions and distinct areas of the cerebellum, largely Lobule VI, Crus 1 and Crus 2 [54–56]. Structural and functional findings are supported by clinical studies reporting that not all cerebellar insults result in motor deficits. In fact, some individuals have minor if any motor impairments and instead suffer from behavioral changes affecting executive functioning, verbal fluency, working memory, abstract reasoning, personality and language [57–59]. Finally, task-based neuroimaging studies have reported increased cerebellar activity in emotional, language, memory and executive tasks [59].

4.1. Conclusions Together, these studies make a clear case that the cerebellum is involved in higher cognitive functions, including those related to emotion and executive functioning. Cerebellar dysfunction may affect reciprocal cerebro-cerebellar pathways and influence regions involved in stimulus processing and avoidance, resulting in individual differences in behavioral inhibition. Continued research of the cerebellar contribution to individual differences observed in behaviorally inhibited temperament will shed important light on the role of the cerebellum in anxiety vulnerability. Acknowledgements The authors would like to thank Dr. Patricia Kenny for her assistance with the study and Theresa Mullarney-Regetz for her efforts in data collection. The authors would also like to thank the Stress and Motivated Behavior Institute and the Rutgers Graduate School of Biomedical Sciences Foundation Scholars Fellowship for financial assistance. References [1] Kessler RC, Berglund P, Demler O, Jin R, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Lifetime prevalence and age-of-onset distributions of DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:593–602. [2] Kessler RC, Chiu WT, Demler O, Merikangas KR, Walters EE. Prevalence, severity, and comorbidity of 12-month DSM-IV disorders in the National Comorbidity Survey Replication. Arch Gen Psychiatry 2005;62:617–27. [3] Mineka S, Zinbarg R. A contemporary learning theory perspective on the etiology of anxiety disorders: it’s not what you thought it was. Am Psychol 2006;61:10–26. [4] Holloway JL, Trivedi P, Myers CE, Servatius RJ. Enhanced conditioned eyeblink response acquisition and proactive interference in anxiety vulnerable individuals. Front Behav Neurosci 2012;6:76. [5] Caulfield MD, McAuley JD, Servatius RJ. Facilitated acquisition of eyeblink conditioning in those vulnerable to anxiety disorders. Front Hum Neurosci 2013:7. [6] Holloway JL, Allen MT, Myers CE, Servatius RJ. Behaviorally inhibited individuals demonstrate significantly enhanced conditioned response acquisition under non-optimal learning conditions. Behav Brain Res 2013;261C:49–55. [7] Biederman J, Rosenbaum JF, Hirshfeld DR, Faraone SV, Bolduc EA, Gersten M, et al. Psychiatric correlates of behavioral inhibition in young children of parents with and without psychiatric disorders. Arch Gen Psychiatry 1990;47: 21–6. [8] Rosenbaum JF, Biederman J, Hirshfeld DR, Bolduc EA, Faraone SV, Kagan J, et al. Further evidence of an association between behavioral inhibition and anxiety disorders: results from a family study of children from a non-clinical sample. J Psychiatr Res 1991;25:49–65. [9] Hirshfeld D, Rosenbaum J, Biederman J. Stable behavioral inhibition and its association with anxiety disorder. J Am Acad Child Adolesc Psychiatry 1992;31:103–11. [10] Kagan J, Moss HA. Birth to maturity: a study in psychological development. Rockville, MD: National Institute of Mental Health; 1962. [11] Garcia Coll C, Kagan J. Behavioral inhibition in young children. Child Dev 1984;55:1005–19. [12] Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N. The physiology and psychology of behavioral inhibition in children. Child Dev 1987;58:1459–73. [13] Schmidt L, Fox N, Rubin K. Behavioral and neuroendocrine responses in shy children. Dev Psychobiol 1997;30:127–40. [14] Kagan J, Reznick JS, Snidman N. Biological bases of childhood shyness. Science 1988;240:167–71. [15] Kagan J. Temperament and the reactions to unfamiliarity. Child Dev 1997;68:139–43. [16] American Psychiatric Association. Diagnostic and statistical manual of mental disorders: DSM-IV-TR. 4th ed. Washington, DC: American Psychiatric Publishing; 2000. [17] Schofield CA, Coles ME, Gibb BE. Retrospective reports of behavioral inhibition and young adults’ current symptoms of social anxiety, depression, and anxious arousal. J Anxiety Disord 2009;23:884–90. [18] Myers CE, VanMeenen KM, McAuley JD, Beck KD, Pang KCH, Servatius RJ. Behaviorally inhibited temperament is associated with severity of post-traumatic stress disorder symptoms and faster eyeblink conditioning in veterans. Stress 2011;15:31–44. [19] Sheynin J, Shikari S, Gluck MA, Moustafa AA, Servatius RJ, Myers CE. Enhanced avoidance learning in behaviorally inhibited young men and women. Stress 2013;16:289–99. [20] Thompson RF. The search for the engram. Am Psychol 1976;31:209–27. [21] Thompson R. The neurobiology of learning and memory. Science 1986;233: 941–7.

M.D. Caulfield et al. / Behavioural Brain Research 278 (2015) 476–481 [22] McCormick DA, Lavond DG, Clark GA, Kettner RE, Rising CE, Thompson RF. The engram found? Role of the cerebellum in classical conditioning of nictitating membrane and eyelid responses. Bull Psychon Soc 1981;18:103–5. [23] Christian KM, Thompson RF. Neural substrates of eyeblink conditioning: acquisition and retention. Learn Mem 2003;10:427–55. [24] Beylin AV, Gandhi CC, Wood GE, Talk AC, Matzel LD, Shors TJ. The role of the hippocampus in trace conditioning: temporal discontinuity or task difficulty? Neurobiol Learn Mem 2001;76:447–61. [25] Little AH, Lipsitt LP, Rovee-Collier C. Classical conditioning and retention of the infant’s eyelid response: effects of age and interstimulus interval. J Exp Child Psychol 1984;37:512–24. [26] Ivkovich D, Collins KL, Eckerman CO, Krasnegor NA, Stanton ME. Classical delay eyeblink conditioning in 4- and 5-month-old human infants. Psychol Sci 1999;10:4–8. [27] Herbert JS, Eckerman CO, Stanton ME. The ontogeny of human learning in delay, long-delay, and trace eyeblink conditioning. Behav Neurosci 2003;117:1196–210. [28] Nicholson DA, Freeman JH. Developmental changes in eyeblink conditioning and simple spike activity in the cerebellar cortex. Dev Psychobiol 2004;44:45–57. [29] Woodruff-Pak DS. Aging and classical conditioning: parallel studies in rabbits and humans. Neurobiol Aging 1988;9:511–22. [30] Solomon PR, Beal MF, Pendlebury WW. Age-related disruption of classical conditioning: a model systems approach to memory disorders. Neurobiol Aging 1988;9:535–46. [31] Solomon PR, Pomerleau D, Bennett L, James J, Morse DL. Acquisition of the classically conditioned eyeblink response in humans over the life span. Psychol Aging 1989;4:34–41. [32] Weiss C, Thompson RF. The effects of age on eyeblink conditioning in the freely moving Fischer-344 rat. Neurobiol Aging 1991;12:249–54. [33] Chiron C, Raynaud C, Mazière B, Zilbovicius M, Laflamme L, Masure MC, et al. Changes in regional cerebral blood flow during brain maturation in children and adolescents. J Nucl Med 1992;33:696–703. [34] Lewis D. Development of the prefrontal cortex during adolescence: insights into vulnerable neural circuits in schizophrenia. Neuropsychopharmacology 1997;16:385–98. [35] Beesdo K, Knappe S, Pine DS. Anxiety and anxiety disorders in children and adolescents: developmental issues and implications for DSM-V. Psychiatr Clin North Am 2009;32:483–524. [36] Ivkovich D, Stanton ME. Effects of early hippocampal lesions on trace, delay, and long-delay eyeblink conditioning in developing rats. Neurobiol Learn Mem 2001;76:426–46. [37] Moustafa AA, Wufong E, Servatius RJ, Pang K. Why trace and delay conditioning are sometimes (but not always) hippocampal dependent: a computational model. Brain Res 2013;1493:48–67. [38] Cheng DT, Disterhoft JF, Power JM, Ellis DA, Desmond JE. Neural substrates underlying human delay and trace eyeblink conditioning. Proc Natl Acad Sci USA 2008;105:8108–13. [39] Cheng DT, Meintjes EM, Stanton ME, Desmond JE, Pienaar M, Dodge NC, et al. Functional MRI of cerebellar activity during eyeblink classical conditioning in children and adults. Hum Brain Mapp 2014;35:1390–403. [40] Lee T, Kim JJ. Differential effects of cerebellar, amygdalar, and hippocampal lesions on classical eyeblink conditioning in rats. J Neurosci 2004;24:3242–50.

481

[41] Barnet RC, Hunt PS. Trace and long-delay fear conditioning in the developing rat. Learn Behav 2005;33:437–43. [42] Ivkovich D, Paczkowski CM, Stanton ME. Ontogeny of delay versus trace eyeblink conditioning in the rat. Dev Psychobiol 2000;36:148–60. [43] Gladstone G, Parker G. Measuring a behaviorally inhibited temperament style: Development and initial validation of new self-report measures. Psychiatry Res 2005;135:133–43. [44] Spielberger CD, Gorsuch R, Lushene R, Vagg PR, Jacobs GA. Manual for the statetrait anxiety inventory (form Y). Palo Alto, CA: Consulting Psychologists Press; 1983. [45] Beck KD, McLaughlin J, Bergen MT, Cominski TP, Moldow RL, Servatius RJ. Facilitated acquisition of the classically conditioned eyeblink response in women taking oral contraceptives. Behav Pharmacol 2008;19:821–8. [46] Solomon PR, Groccia-Ellison ME. Classic conditioning in aged rabbits: delay, trace, and long-delay conditioning. Behav Neurosci 1996;110: 427–35. [47] Thompson LT, Moyer JR, Disterhoft JF. Trace eyeblink conditioning in rabbits demonstrates heterogeneity of learning ability both between and within age groups. Neurobiol Aging 1996;17:619–29. [48] Gilbertson MW, Paulus LA, Williston SK, Gurvits TV, Lasko NB, Pitman RK, et al. Neurocognitive function in monozygotic twins discordant for combat exposure: relationship to posttraumatic stress disorder. J Abnorm Psychol 2006;115:484–95. [49] Blackford JU, Allen AH, Cowan RL, Avery SN. Amygdala and hippocampus fail to habituate to faces in individuals with an inhibited temperament. Soc Cogn Affect Neurosci 2013;8:143–50. [50] Blackford JU, Avery SN, Shelton RC, Zald DH. Amygdala temporal dynamics: temperamental differences in the timing of amygdala response to familiar and novel faces. BMC Neurosci 2009;10:145. [51] Buckner RL. The cerebellum and cognitive function: 25 years of insight from anatomy and neuroimaging. Neuron 2013;80:807–15. [52] Middleton FA, Strick PL. Cerebellar projections to the prefrontal cortex of the primate. J Neurosci 2001;21:700–12. [53] Bostan AC, Dum RP, Strick PL. Cerebellar networks with the cerebral cortex and basal ganglia. Trends Cogn Sci 2013;17:241–54. [54] Habas C, Kamdar N, Nguyen D, Prater K, Beckmann CF, Menon V, et al. Distinct cerebellar contributions to intrinsic connectivity networks. J Neurosci 2009;29:8586–94. [55] O’Reilly JX, Beckmann CF, Tomassini V, Ramnani N, Johansen-Berg H. Distinct and overlapping functional zones in the cerebellum defined by resting state functional connectivity. Cereb Cortex 2010;20:953–65. [56] Bernard JA, Seidler RD, Hassevoort KM, Benson BL, Welsh RC, Wiggins JL, et al. Resting state cortico-cerebellar functional connectivity networks: a comparison of anatomical and self-organizing map approaches. Front Neuroanat 2012;6:31. [57] Schmahmann JD, Macmore J, Vangel M. Cerebellar stroke without motor deficit: clinical evidence for motor and non-motor domains within the human cerebellum. Neuroscience 2009;162:852–61. [58] Schmahmann JD, Sherman JC. The cerebellar cognitive affective syndrome. Brain 1998;121(Pt 4):561–79. [59] E K-H, Chen S-HA, Ho M-HR, Desmond JE. A meta-analysis of cerebellar contributions to higher cognition from PET and fMRI studies. Hum Brain Mapp 2014;35:593–615.

Facilitated acquisition of standard but not long delay classical eyeblink conditioning in behaviorally inhibited adolescents.

Adolescence is a key age in the development of anxiety disorders. The present study assessed the relationship between behavioral inhibition, a risk fa...
816KB Sizes 0 Downloads 4 Views