CYBERPSYCHOLOGY, BEHAVIOR, AND SOCIAL NETWORKING Volume 17, Number 9, 2014 ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. DOI: 10.1089/cyber.2014.0173

Effects of a Brief School-Based Media Literacy Intervention on Digital Media Use in Adolescents: Cluster Randomized Controlled Trial Birte Walther, PhD,1 Reiner Hanewinkel, PhD,1,2 and Matthis Morgenstern, PhD1,2

Abstract

The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects of a four-session school-based media literacy curriculum on adolescent computer gaming and Internet use behavior. The study comprised a cluster randomized controlled trial with three assessments (baseline, posttest, and 12-month follow-up). At baseline, a total of 2,303 sixth and seventh grade adolescents from 27 secondary schools were assessed. Of these, 1,843 (80%) could be reached at all three assessments (Mage = 12.0 years; SD = 0.83). Students of the intervention group received the media literacy program Vernetzte www.Welten (‘‘Connected www.Worlds’’) implemented by trained teachers during class time. The control group attended regular class. Main outcome measures were adolescents’ computer gaming and Internet use: days per month, hours per day, and addictive use patterns. Parental media monitoring and rules at home were assessed as secondary outcomes. Results of multilevel growth-curve models revealed a significant intervention effect in terms of a lower increase in self-reported gaming frequency (b = - 1.10 [95% CI - 2.06, - 0.13]), gaming time (b = - 0.27 [95% CI - 0.40, - 0.14]), and proportion of excessive gamers (AOR = 0.21 [95% CI 0.08, 0.57]) in the intervention group. There were also significant group–time interactions for the addictive gaming scale (b = - 0.08 [95% CI - 0.12, - 0.04]), and the Internet Addiction Scale (b = - 0.06 [95% CI - 0.10, - 0.01]). No effect was found for days and hours of Internet use or parental media behavior. The study shows that the program Vernetzte www.Welten can influence adolescents’ media use behavior. Future research should address mediating and moderating variables of program effects.

Introduction

T

he relation between digital media use and adolescent mental and physical health is a topic of concern for parents, teachers, and researchers worldwide.1 Much of the scientific discussion is about potential detrimental effects of media use on youth adjustment and health, while other studies look at media’s potential as a tool to promote positive health behaviors of adolescents.2 There is empirical evidence that exposure to specific media images and messages is a risk factor for aggressive behaviors,3 risk-taking,4 substance use,5,6 eating disorders and overweight,7,8 cyberbullying,9 impaired sleep,10 or misperceptions of sexuality and body norms.11–13 Another health-related risk of media use is excessive use of digital media itself. Several studies have shown that video gamers or Internet users can develop use patterns that can be characterized as addictive.14–16 Given the risks associated with the use of digital screen media and given the ubiquitous nature of media in general, 1 2

the question arises how children and adolescents can be protected from harmful media effects. There seems to be consensus that a complete ban of adolescent media use cannot be a realistic or adequate preventive effort, as media are an inevitable part of children and adolescents’ life today. Existing prevention approaches can be classified into reduction and immunization approaches. Reduction approaches are based on a dose–response media effect model, and assume that less exposure is better than more. Immunization approaches assume that adolescents can be protected against the risks of harmful media effects by enhancing their (and their parents’) media literacy, that is, they can be trained to use media and to process media contents in a healthier way.17 These two approaches are not mutually exclusive. Enhancing media literacy can also lead to a reduction in exposure, for example by changing parental media behavior or by changing social norms regarding media use. Such interventions can be an effective way to improve media knowledge and critical thinking, as well as to change

Institute for Therapy and Health Research (IFT-Nord), Kiel, Germany. Institute for Medical Psychology and Medical Sociology, University Medical Center Schleswig-Holstein, Kiel, Germany.

616

MEDIA LITERACY INTERVENTION

health-related problematic attitudes and behaviors.18–24 However, primary outcomes of previous studies were specific health behaviors not excessive screen times or proxies of addictive use patterns.25 The aim of the present study was to close this gap by developing and testing a short schoolbased curriculum on media literacy that focuses on problematic use of the Internet and computer games. Materials and Methods Study design

A two-arm cluster randomized control trial with three assessments (baseline/posttest/follow-up) was conducted. Intervention

Vernetzte www.Welten (‘‘connected www.worlds’’) is a school-based media literacy program targeting adolescents in 6th and 7th grade. It is implemented by teachers during class time and consists of four 90-minute units: Internet use (unit 1), online communication (unit 2), gaming (unit 3), and gambling (unit 4). The focus lies on self-monitoring, discussion, and reflection of adolescents’ media use. An important aspect is that adolescents learn to apply the concept of behavioral addictions. Unit 1 starts with a self-reflection of the students’ own use patterns, including a discussion about which kinds of use pattern could already be viewed as problematic. Subsequent activities deal with the validity of online content, aiming at adolescents’ critical perception and interpretation of different types of online information (e.g., Wikisystems, personal Web sites, and advertisements). Unit 2 also starts with a selfreflection and a discussion of the students’ online communication behavior. It deals with potential risks of different types and situations of online communication (e.g., data protection). The gaming unit contains activities to reflect gaming preferences, motives, and gaming times. Also, the age restriction of video games is explained. On the basis of a comic strip about a dog becoming addicted to video gaming, the concept of gaming addiction is discussed in detail. The program also contains a booklet for parents that provides recommendations on adolescent media times, and encourages the exchange of media-related views and experiences with their children. The program materials are freely available at www.vernetztewelten.ift-nord.de. Study procedure

In August 2010, 80 randomly selected secondary schools from the German Federal State of Schleswig-Holstein were invited to participate with all their 6th and 7th grade classes in a study on media literacy. Nineteen schools agreed to join the study. Blockwise randomization was performed at school level, stratified by school type (more detailed information regarding the randomization procedure is given elsewhere).26 After randomization, schools were informed about their group status (intervention/control) and were requested to register all participating classes. Because schools from the intervention group registered more classes than schools from the control group, 39 additional schools were invited for the control condition to counteract this sample size bias (Fig. 1). Eight of these schools agreed to participate. Parents received written information about the study with the opportunity to

617

refuse their child’s participation. Between December 2010 and February 2011, the program was implemented in the intervention group by trained teachers. Post-test and followup data were collected in May 2011 and 2012 respectively. All data were collected through self-completion questionnaires. To link individual data of all assessments without violating anonymity, each questionnaire was labeled with a seven-digit code self-generated by each student.27 Design and procedure were approved by the Ethics Committee of the Medical Faculty of the University of Kiel, the Ministry of Education, and the Data Protection Commissioner of Schleswig-Holstein. Sample

Twenty-seven schools (22.7%) agreed to join the study and registered 102 classes with a total of 2,494 students. From all eligible students, 4.5% (n = 112) were withdrawn by their parents, and 3.2% (n = 79) were absent on the day of the baseline data collection. The retention rate at posttest was 93.4% (n = 2,154). Of these, 87.2% (n = 1,879) could be reached again at follow-up (see Fig. 1). Thirty-six cases were excluded from the analyses because two intervention classes (n = 34) did not implement the program and two students gave inconsistent responses. The final sample consisted of 1,843 students with complete data at all three measurements (total dropout rate: 20.0%). Measures Gaming behavior. Adolescents were asked how often they usually play computer, video, or online games (never/ less than once per month/once per month/several times per month/once per week/daily) and how long they usually play for (none/0.5/1/2/2–4/4–8/ > 8 hours). Addictive gaming was approached with six items of the German Computer Gaming Addiction Scale (KFN-CSAS-II):28 ‘‘My thoughts continually circle around playing video games, even when I’m not playing’’; ‘‘At certain times or in certain situations, I actually always play: That has almost become a routine for me’’; ‘‘My school achievement suffers from my gaming habits’’; ‘‘People important to me complain that I spend too much time playing’’; ‘‘I have a feeling that video games are getting more and more important for me’’; ‘‘If I can’t play, I am irritable and dissatisfied’’ (4-point rating scale). A scale mean was calculated across all items (range 0–3; a = 0.82). Internet use. Frequency of Internet use was measured with ‘‘How many days per week do you use the Internet?’’ (none/1/2/3/4/5/6/7 days). Students also had to estimate how long they usually stay online on a weekday (response options: none/0.5/1/2/2–4/4–8/ > 8 hours). Addictive Internet use was measured by six items of the German Internet Addiction Scale (ISS):29 ‘‘When surfing the Internet, I often catch myself saying: Just another few minutes. And then, however, I cannot stop’’; ‘‘My thoughts continually circle around the Internet, even when I’m not online’’; ‘‘If I’m not online for quite a while, I become restless and nervous’’; ‘‘Online activities do increasingly influence my everyday life’’; ‘‘People important to me complain that I spend too much time online’’; and ‘‘My school achievement suffers under my online habits’’ (4-point rating scale). Responses

618

FIG. 1.

WALTHER ET AL.

Study flow chart.

were again summed up and divided by the number of items (range 0–3; a = 0.79). Parental monitoring and rules at home. Adolescents were asked how much their parents know about their online activities and about the games they play (5-point rating scale). Further, adolescents had to estimate how often they talked with their parents about time and content of their media use in the past 3 months (never/once/more than once/ more than four times). Media rules at home were assessed by: ‘‘Are there rules regarding the use of Internet and computer games at your home?’’ (yes/no) and ‘‘How much do your parents care that these rules are met?’’ (not at all/little/ strong/very strong/there are no rules). Covariates. The following covariates were assessed, as these have been frequently reported to be associated with media use: age, gender, migration background, socioeconomic status (SES), and access to a computer and the Internet at

home. The SES was measured by two items of the Family Affluence Scale30 (‘‘Does your family own a car?’’ [no/ yes, one/ yes, two or more] and ‘‘During the past 12 months, how many times did you travel away on holiday with your family?’’ [not at all/once/twice/more than twice]). An unweighted mean score (range 0–2.5) was calculated, with higher scores indicating a higher SES. Migration background of the students was approximated by asking for the father’s and mother’s country of birth (Germany/any other). For the analyses, the migration variable was dichotomized into ‘‘both parents born in Germany’’ versus ‘‘else.’’ Access to a computer, laptop, and the Internet was measured with three items (not available/ shared use with parents or siblings/I have my own). An unweighted mean score (range 0–2) was calculated across all three items, with a higher score indicating higher access. Statistical analyses

All analyses were conducted with Stata 13. Baseline differences between the control group and the intervention

MEDIA LITERACY INTERVENTION

619

group were tested using chi square and t tests. To test intervention effects over time, multilevel growth-curve models with maximum likelihood estimation were applied. For dichotomous outcomes, logistic models were used. All models included random intercepts for class and individual to account for the nonindependence of observations. Frequency of gaming and Internet use were transferred into days per month (continuous) and daily users (dichotomous). Time/duration of gaming and Internet use were transferred into hours per day (continuous) and excessive users (q4 hours per day, dichotomous). Because of differences between the intervention and control group in terms of age, gender, school type, socioeconomic status, and access to a computer and the Internet, these variables were included as fixed effects in the analyses. Results Attrition analyses

We compared the baseline characteristics of those who dropped out of the study or did not implement the intervention (n = 460) with those who completed the survey on all three occasions (n = 1,843). Lost students were significantly older, of lower SES, had a higher average computer gaming time, and were less often recruited in Gymnasiums ( p < 0.001). The drop-out rate was not significantly different in the control (20.4%) and the intervention group (19.4%), and we found also no attrition–group interaction for any of the assessed variables, indicating that there was no differential attrition. Baseline equivalence between intervention and control group

The mean age of the final study sample was 12.0 years (SD = 0.83) with a range of 10 to 14 years. About half of the sample (50.5%) was female. The two conditions were tested for baseline differences on all measured variables. Despite randomization, there was evidence for inequality between the control and the intervention group with regard to four measures. Compared to the control condition, students in the intervention group reported slightly lower age, lower SES, less access to a computer and the Internet, and more often visited the Haupt- or Realschule (see Table 1).

Gaming, Internet use, and parental behavior

Table 2 presents the unadjusted frequencies and means for all primary endpoints of the study, dependent on group condition and time of data assessment. At this descriptive level, it can be seen that there was a decrease in gaming frequency and duration from baseline (time 1) to posttest (time 2) in the intervention group and an increase in gaming frequency from posttest (time 2) to the follow-up (time 3) assessment in both groups. The crude numbers suggest that the increase in the gaming measures from time 2 to time 3 was less pronounced in the intervention group. There were also small changes over time in the gaming addiction scale means, displaying a similar time pattern in terms of a higher decrease in the intervention group from time 1 to time 2, followed by a lower increase from time 2 to time 3. Generally, low means on the gaming addiction scale indicate that zero (rejection of the items) was by far the most common response on this scale. Use of the Internet also increased over time, with the proportion of daily Internet users almost doubling within the study interval (15 months). However, except for excessive Internet use (q4 hours per day) and the ISS means, there was no visible difference in the development of frequency and duration between the two groups in the crude numbers. At baseline, 42.3% of the students reported that their parents know ‘‘very much’’ about the computer games they play, and 28.5% reported ‘‘very high’’ parental knowledge about their use of the Internet. These numbers decreased over time, with 36.1% reporting ‘‘very much’’ knowledge of parents regarding computer games and 18.3% regarding Internet content. This development was the same in both study groups. Three-fourths of the students confirmed that they have rules at home regarding computer and Internet use. This percentage also decreased over time (at follow-up: 62.3%), but again there was no visible difference in numbers between the intervention and control group. Multivariate test of program effects

Adjusted multilevel analyses confirmed a differential development of the two groups’ gaming behavior over time

Table 1. Sample Characteristics at Baseline Intervention (n = 804)

Gender Male Female School type Gymnasium1 Gemeinschafts- and Gesamtschule Haupt-, Real-, and Regionalschule Age SES (range 0–2.5) Access to a computer and the Internet (range 0–2)

Control (n = 1.039)

N

%

N

%

v2 (df)

p

381 423

47.4 52.6

531 508

51.1 48.9

2.51 (1)

0.11

505 84 215

62.8 10.5 26.7

718 166 155

69.1 16.0 14.9

44.48 (2)

< 0.001

M 11.8 1.48 1.36

SD .80 .65 .44

M 12.1 1.59 1.41

SD .83 .64 .40

t 7.97 3.50 2.65

(df) (1,840) < 0.001 (1,840) < 0.001 (1,840) < 0.001

1 Gymnasium is a German school type finishing with a university-entrance diploma after grade 12 or 13. Hauptschule, Realschule, and Regionalschule focus on students with lower academic skills finishing after grade 9 or 10. At Gemeinschaftsschule and Gesamtschule, students with varying skills are taught together offering all kinds of degrees.

620

WALTHER ET AL.

Table 2. Outcomes by Group and Time Point Gaming frequency (days per month)

Daily gamers

Gaming time (hours per day)

Excessive gamers (q4 hours per day)

Gaming addiction scale (range 0–3)

Mean

%

Mean

%

Mean

8.14 6.96 8.04

14.4 12.1 16.9

1.06 0.99 1.18

3.7 2.8 5.5

0.38 0.28 0.35

8.36 7.80 9.35

14.6 14.6 21.8

0.95 1.06 1.29

2.0 4.6 6.2

0.39 0.36 0.45

Internet use frequency (days per month)

Daily Internet use

Internet use time (hours per day)

Excessive Internet use (q4 hours per day)

Internet addiction scale (range 0–3)

Mean

%

Mean

%

Mean

16.5 18.3 22.2

21.0 27.0 43.6

1.08 1.26 1.73

3.1 3.9 7.6

0.36 0.34 0.45

17.7 19.1 22.8

24.1 29.7 45.5

1.05 1.27 1.79

2.5 4.6 9.7

0.39 0.41 0.53

Time Intervention 1 2 3 Control 1 2 3

Time Intervention 1 2 3 Control 1 2 3

(see Table 3). The repeated measures regressions revealed significant time–group interactions for all gaming measures, indicating that the lower or nondecrease of gaming in the control group at time 2 and the higher increase of gaming at time 3 were above chance level. No program effect could be detected for Internet use frequency and duration (Table 4). But there was a significant time–group interaction on the ISS, showing a pronounced increase on the scale mean for the control group. No systematic intervention effect was found for the student reports on parental media monitoring and rules at home. Discussion

This study examined the effects of a brief school-based media literacy intervention. Analyses showed evidence for intervention control differences in the 15 month development of computer gaming frequency and gaming time per day, with intervention students indicating a lower increase in both absolute gaming days and hours, as well as proportions of daily and excessive gaming sessions. In addition, we also found differential developments in mean responses on items of two scales assessing addictive media use: the KFN-CSAS and ISS. No intervention effects were found for frequency and duration of Internet use or for students’ reports of parental monitoring or rules about media behavior at home. To our knowledge, this is the first study on a school-based media literacy program targeting actual computer gaming and Internet use behavior. The present findings are promising, given the shortness and economy of the program, even though the positive effects were only found for computer gaming and not for Internet use times. This lack of finding for Internet behavior was not hypothesized and is not obvious from the content of the intervention. One explanation is that it is a measurement issue. While computer gaming is a well-defined

activity that can easily be reported, Internet use is much more multifaceted. ‘‘To be online’’ could mean to surf the Internet for information, to listen to Internet radio, to watch television, to use social media, or even just having a smartphone in the pocket that is connected. Thus, quantifying ones Internet use might be a difficult task, and the assessment is therefore more error-prone.31 Another possible explanation is that Internet use is such a routine in the daily lives of today’s adolescents that reducing its frequency or duration is just harder to accomplish than for computer gaming. Differences in the problematic/addictive media use measures might need some further consideration, as one can question if these small differences in scale means, even if significant, have any clinical significance. Translated into raw numbers of ‘‘addicted gamers,’’ the results indicate an increase from 9 to 25 ‘‘addicted gamers’’ in the control group, compared to a decrease from 11 to 7 in the intervention group. Comparable numbers for ‘‘addicted Internet use’’ (according to ISS cutoffs) were a net increase of 14 (from 8 to 22) addicted users among controls compared to a net increase of 6 (from 8 to 14) ‘‘addicted users’’ in the intervention condition. Hence, even if the relative proportional differences are very small, the absolute difference seems relevant, given the low prevalence of addictive gaming and addicted Internet use in the German youth population.28,32 However, there always remains the alternative explanation that changes in responses on clinical scales reflect changes in knowledge about ‘‘addictive use’’ rather than changes in actual ‘‘addictive use.’’ This is especially true for adolescent samples. Another question is related to the mediators of the intervention effect. In theory, we assume that a media literacy intervention enhances media literacy, which is, in turn, also the mediating variable. However, we do not have valid data to test this assumption. One problem of the concept ‘‘media

0.71 1.89

0.71 0.60*

1.78 2.13

1.02

AOR

[0.52, 0.96] [1.67, 2.13]

[0.43, 1.16] [0.37, 0.98]*

[0.55, 5.77] [0.69, 6.59]

[0.61, 1.69]

95% CI

Daily gaming

0.27 0.82

- 0.19* - 0.27*

0.10 0.53*

0.11

b

[0.21, 0.34] [0.78, 0.86]

[- 0.32, - 0.06]* [- 0.40, - 0.14]*

[- 0.17, 0.38] [0.26, 0.81]*

[- 0.06, 0.27]

95% CI

Gaming time

0.83 2.38

0.14* 0.21*

5.58 26.92*

3.10*

0.50 2.03

0.98 1.09

2.58 11.69*

0.90

AOR

[0.35, 0.73] [1.84, 2.23]

[0.65, 1.49] [0.72, 1.63]

[0.99, 6.76] [4.62, 29.57]*

[0.59, 1.39]

95% CI

Daily Internet use

0.28 0.88

- 0.05 - 0.09

0.26 0.74*

0.04

b

[0.22, 0.37] [0.83, 0.92]

[- 0.19, 0.09] [- 0.23, 0.06]

[- 0.05, 0.56] [0.44, 1.05]*

[- 0.14, 0.22]

95% CI

Internet time

0.97 2.11

0.50 0.44

11.27 55.48*

1.48

AOR

[0.68, 1.37] [1.74, 2.56]

[0.19, 1.27] [0.18, 1.04]

[0.96, 132.43] [5.58, 551.64]*

[0.59, 3.72]

95% CI

Excessive Internet use

*Significant at p < 0.05. Statistically controlled for gender, age, SES, school type and access to a computer and the Internet. 1 Control group coded ‘‘0’’ and intervention group coded ‘‘1.’’

[1.37, 2.39] [6.14, 6.71]

Random intercepts Class level 1.81 Student level 6.42

[2.51, 6.15]* [7.67, 11.30]*

[- 1.57, 0.72]

[- 0.94, 0.77] [- 0.84, 0.86]

4.33* 9.49*

- 0.42

95% CI

- 0.08 0.01

Fixed effects Group1 1 Time 2 3 Time–group 21 31

b

Internet use frequency

Table 4. Results of Multilevel Mixed Effect Regressions for Internet Use

[0.50, 1.38] [1.92, 2.96]

[0.05, 0.40]* [0.08, 0.57]*

[0.39, 79.57] [2.28, 318.50]*

[1.17, 8.26]*

95% CI

Excessive gaming AOR

*Significant at p < 0.05. Statistically controlled for gender, age, SES, school type, and access to a computer and the Internet. 1 Control group coded ‘‘0’’ and intervention group coded ‘‘1.’’

[1.43, 2.34] [5.00, 5.58]

[- 1.72, 0.20] [- 2.06, - 0.13]*

[- 0.86, 3.25] [- 0.95, 3.16]

1.19 1.11

- 0.76 - 1.10*

[- 1.27, 1.00]

95% CI

- 0.14

Random intercepts Class level 1.83 Student level 5.28

Fixed effects Group1 1 Time 2 3 Time–group 21 31

b

Gaming frequency

Table 3. Results of Multilevel Mixed Effect Regressions for Gaming

0.07 0.32

- 0.04 - 0.06*

0.10* 0.33*

0.01

b

[0.05, 0.10] [0.28, 0.31]

[- 0.11, - 0.03]* [- 0.12, - 0.04]*

[0.03, 0.20]* [0.10, 0.28]*

[- 0.05, 0.05]

95% CI

[0.05, 0.10] [0.31, 0.34]

[- 0.09, 0.00] [- 0.10, - 0.01]*

[0.01, 0.19]* [0.24, 0.43]*

[- 0.05, 0.05]

95% CI

Internet addiction

0.07 0.29

- 0.07* - 0.08*

0.12* 0.19*

- 0.00

b

Gaming addiction

MEDIA LITERACY INTERVENTION 621

622

literacy’’ is that it is very broad and ambiguous, and hence there is no agreed-upon way of how it is best measured.33 Especially knowledge-based measures of media literacy face the difficulty that adolescents with high media use usually also have higher media knowledge, making it an unlikely mediating variable in a media reduction intervention study. However, after looking at the sheer contents of the four sessions, there is support for the idea that one successful prevention effort might be the change of social norms regarding media use frequency.1 There was no indication that the found results were only based on structural changes such as changes in parental rule setting. There are further limitations of the current study. First, despite randomization, there were baseline differences between the two groups on several measures. It cannot be excluded that there were additional differences between the groups that were not assessed, a fact that reduces internal validity of the study. Second, there was a self-selection process on school level in advance of the randomization, which impairs the external validity of the study. Nevertheless, this study is one of the few trials on the effectiveness of a media literacy intervention and the first to address the prevention of excessive computer and Internet use in adolescents. Results show that changes in media use behavior are possible with comparatively little effort. Future research will be needed to validate these data and to shed further light on mediating and moderating variables.

WALTHER ET AL.

6.

7.

8. 9. 10. 11.

12. 13.

Acknowledgments

This research was supported by the Ministry of Social Affaires, Health, Family and Equal Opportunities of the Land Schleswig-Holstein. We would like to thank Uli Tondorf for his advice and support in the development of the intervention. We also thank Nadia El Bouhayati, Svenja Cleve, Lars Grabbe, Andre Lischick, Serdar Peker, and Wiebke Pustal for assessing the data. Author Disclosure Statement

No competing financial interests exist.

14. 15. 16. 17. 18.

References

1. Strasburger VC, Jordan AB, Donnerstein E. Health effects of media on children and adolescents. Pediatrics 2010; 125: 756–767. 2. Hieftje K, Edelman EJ, Camenga DR, et al. Electronic media-based health interventions promoting behavior change in youth: a systematic review. JAMA Pediatrics 2013; 167: 574–580. 3. Bushman BJ, Huesmann LR. Short-term and long-term effects of violent media on aggression in children and adults. Archives of Pediatrics & Adolescent Medicine 2006; 160: 348–352. 4. Fischer P, Greitemeyer T, Kastenmu¨ller A, et al. The effects of risk-glorifying media exposure on risk-positive cognitions, emotions, and behaviors: a meta-analytic review. Psychological Bulletin 2011; 137:367–390. 5. U.S. Department of Health and Human Services. (2012) Preventing tobacco use among youth and young adults: A report of the Surgeon General. Atlanta, GA: U.S. Department of Health and Human Services, Centers for Disease Control

19. 20. 21. 22. 23. 24.

and Prevention, National Center for Chronic Disease Prevention and Health Promotion, Office on Smoking and Health. Smith LA, Foxcroft DR. The effect of alcohol advertising, marketing and portrayal on drinking behaviour in young people: systematic review of prospective cohort studies. BMC Public Health 2009; 9:51. Stice E, Schupak-Neuberg E, Shaw HE, et al. Relation of media exposure to eating disorder symptomatology: an examination of mediating mechanisms. Journal of Abnormal Psychology 1994; 103:836–840. Melchior M, Chollet A, Fombonne E, et al. Internet and video game use in relation to overweight in young adults. American Journal of Health Promotion 2014; 28:321–324. Kowalski RM, Limber SP. Electronic bullying among middle school students. Journal of Adolescent Health 2007; 41:S22–S30. Garrison MM, Liekweg K, Christakis DA. Media use and child sleep: the impact of content, timing, and environment. Pediatrics 2011; 128:29–35. Pinkleton BE, Austin EW, Chen YC, et al. The role of media literacy in shaping adolescents’ understanding of and responses to sexual portrayals in mass media. Journal of Health Communication 2012; 17:460–476. Noll JG, Shenk CE, Barnes JE, et al. Association of maltreatment with high-risk Internet behaviors and offline encounters. Pediatrics 2013; 131:e510–e517. Grabe S, Ward LM, Hyde JS. The role of the media in body image concerns among women: a meta-analysis of experimental and correlational studies. Psychological Bulletin 2008; 134:460–476. Gentile DA, Choo H, Liau A, et al. Pathological video game use among youths: a two-year longitudinal study. Pediatrics 2011; 127:e319–e329. Kuss DJ, Griffiths MD. Online gaming addiction in children and adolescents: a review of empirical research. Journal of Behavioral Addictions 2012; 1:3–22. Young KS. Internet addiction: a new clinical phenomenon and its consequences. American Behavioral Scientist 2004; 48:402–415. Strasburger VC. Pediatricians, schools, and media. Pediatrics 2012; 129:1161–1163. Bergsma LJ, Carney ME. Effectiveness of health-promoting media literacy education: a systematic review. Health Education Research 2008; 23:522–542. Chen YC. The effectiveness of different approaches to media literacy in modifying adolescents’ responses to alcohol. Journal of Health Communication 2013; 18:723–739. Christakis DA, Garrison MM, Herrenkohl T, et al. Modifying media content for preschool children: a randomized controlled trial. Pediatrics 2013; 131:431–438. Fingar KR, Jolls T. Evaluation of a school-based violence prevention media literacy curriculum. Injury Prevention 2014; 20:183–190. Jeong SH, Cho H, Hwang Y. Media literacy interventions: a meta-analytic review. Journal of Communication 2012; 62:454–472. Kupersmidt JB, Scull TM, Austin EW. Media literacy education for elementary school substance use prevention: study of media detective. Pediatrics 2010; 126:525–531. Wilksch SM, Wade TD. Reduction of shape and weight concern in young adolescents: a 30-month controlled evaluation of a media literacy program. Journal of the American Academy of Child & Adolescent Psychiatry 2009; 48:652–661.

MEDIA LITERACY INTERVENTION

25. Kwon JH. (2010) Toward the prevention of adolescent Internet addiction. In Young KS, de Abreu CN, eds. Internet addiction. A handbook and guide to evaluation and treatment. Hoboken, NJ: Wiley, pp. 223–244. 26. Walther B, Hanewinkel R, Morgenstern M. Short-term effects of a school-based program on gambling prevention in adolescents. Journal of Adolescent Health 2013; 52:599– 605. 27. Galanti MR, Siliquini R, Cuomo L, et al. Testing anonymous link procedures for follow-up of adolescents in a school-based trial: the EU-DAP pilot study. Preventive Medicine 2007; 44:174–177. 28. Rehbein F, Kleimann M, Mo¨ßle T. Prevalence and risk factors of video game dependency in adolescence: results of a German nationwide survey. Cyberpsychology, Behavior, & Social Networking 2010; 13:269–277. 29. Hahn A, Jerusalem M. (2001) [Internet addiction—reliability and validity in online research]. In Theobald A, Dreyer M, Starsetzki T, eds. Online-Marktforschung: Beitra¨ge aus Wissenschaft und Praxis. Wiesbaden: Gabler, pp. 211–234 [in German]. 30. Currie CE, Elton RA, Todd J, et al. Indicators of socioeconomic status for adolescents: the WHO Health Beha-

623

viour in School-aged Children Survey. Health Education Researcg 1997; 12:385–397. 31. Moreno MA, Jelenchik LA, Koff R, et al. Associations between Internet use and fitness among college students: an experience sampling approach. Journal of Interaction Science 2013; 1:1–8. 32. Peukert P, Sieslack S, Barth G, et al. [Internet and computer game addiction: phenomenology, comorbidity, etiology, diagnostics and therapeutic implications for the addictives and their relatives]. Psychiatrische Praxis 2010; 37:219– 224 [in German]. 33. Arke ET, Primack BA. Quantifying media literacy: development, reliability, and validity of a new measure. Educational Media International 2009; 46:53–65.

Address correspondence to: Dr. Matthis Morgenstern Institute for Therapy and Health Research (IFT-Nord) Harmsstraße 2 24114 Kiel Germany E-mail: [email protected]

Effects of a brief school-based media literacy intervention on digital media use in adolescents: cluster randomized controlled trial.

The aim of this study was to evaluate the effects of a four-session school-based media literacy curriculum on adolescent computer gaming and Internet ...
275KB Sizes 0 Downloads 6 Views