Original Article

ZEBRAFISH Volume 00, Number 00, 2014 ª Mary Ann Liebert, Inc. DOI: 10.1089/zeb.2014.0977

Effects of the Number of Subjects on the Dark/Light Preference of Zebrafish (Danio rerio) Bruno de Matos Mansur,1 Bruno Rodrigues dos Santos,1 Cla´udio Alberto Gellis de Mattos Dias,2 Marcelo de Sena Pinheiro,1 and Amauri Gouveia Jr.1

Abstract

This research aims to describe the effects of a variable number of Danio rerio fish subjects, ranging from one to eight, in the light/dark box preference test. Four hundred eighty adult male short-finned phenotype zebrafish were tested in the light/dark box. There were four groups in this experiment and a different number of subjects was used in each group: the control group had only one subject, whereas the experimental groups had either two, four, or eight subjects simultaneously inside the apparatus in every session. The average occurrence (AO) of subjects in the white side of the aquarium and the first choice average (FC) were recorded. The AO revealed no difference between the control group and test groups with two and four subjects. The results for the test group with eight subjects showed significant difference when compared to the control group and from the test group with two subjects. The FC also showed no difference between the control group and test groups with two and four subjects. There was significant variation between the control and the test group with eight subjects. The results reflect a conflict between the animal’s preference for dark places and the innate drive to explore new environments. Zebrafish are highly social animals, exhibiting preference for swimming in groups and other patterns of social cohesion. The reduced white avoidance behavior in the test group of eight subjects may possibly reflect the role of shoaling, which is a defensive behavior, in reducing anxiety and stress. On the other hand, the absence of difference between the control group and test groups with two and four subjects suggest that it is feasible to run the light/dark test with up to four subjects, becoming an alternative to streamline and simplify data collection and test analysis. et al.,8 scototaxis can be used as an indicator of stress, fear, and anxiety in zebrafish, which can be measured and exploited through the light/dark box test.

Introduction

Z

ebrafish (Danio rerio) originated in the valleys of India and Bangladesh,1 and has been widely used in behavioral research due to its small size, ease of use, breeding, and keeping. It has also shown potential to be used as a model in social behavior2 research. The zebrafish is well adapted to life in aquariums.3 Diurnal4 fish live in clean and clear water in ditches or shallow lakes, and in between vegetation and rice paddies.5 Consequently, the behavior of hiding in dark places to avoid predators is a requirement to survive in these well-lit environments. Scototaxis, which is the preference for dark environments as opposed to bright ones,6 is a natural strategy that promotes the survival of animals in this environment, constituting a particular defensive pattern of species that exploit concealment or lack of illumination in the environment to avoid predators.7,8 This behavior may be an indicator of fear and anxiety in several species, including zebrafish.9,10 According to Maximino 1 2

Anxiety and the light/dark test

Tests like the light/dark test are based on novelty paradigms and are commonly used in behavioral neuroscience to study fear and anxiety.11 According to McNaughton and Corr,12 anxiety is most often generated by concurrent and equivalent activation of fear and risk assessment systems. The particular set of responses depends on the perceived threat.13–16 Thus, risk assessment behaviors, like thigmotaxis (swimming next to walls), stretching posture to observe, and different exploratory distribution in threatening and safe situations are seen in situations where the risk is low.17 This can be observed in tests like the open field, the elevated plus maze, and the light/dark box. The light/dark test has gained behavioral validity for some fish species (Cardinal-tetras Paracheirodon axelrodi; lambaris

Universidade Federal do Para´, Bele´m, Brazil. Universidade Federal do Amapa´, Macapa´, Brazil.

1

2

Astyanax altiparanae; Nile tilapias Oreochromis niloticus; guppies Poecilia reticulata; and banded-knife fish Gymnotus carapo), including zebrafish.6 To establish pharmacological validity, the light/dark test with the Danio rerio8 species used several drugs, such as lysergic acid diethylamide (LSD),18 acute or chronic fluoxetine, buspirone, benzodiazepines, moclobemide, caffeine, ethanol, and dizocilpine.19 Other variables, like the effects of the forced exposition to a white chamber, lightness level (250 and 500 lux),20 effects of alarm substance,21 methylmercury,22 and the effects of circadian cycle,23 have also been tested. The results have altogether demonstrated that this method can be useful to assess anxietylike behaviors in Danio rerio, showing consistency in the dark preference behavioral pattern, which is responsive to test manipulations. Such findings render the model a large face, use and predictive validity as an animal model of anxiety for this species. Shoal

Zebrafish are highly social and prefer swimming in groups. Social cohesion in zebrafish is exhibited through social interactions, shoaling, boldness, aggression, and social preference.24,25 Being part of a group helps to avoid predators, to travel efficiently, to acquire food,26 and to reproduce more successfully.27 According to Spence et al.,4 forming schools is a natural zebrafish behavior and shoaling initiates subsequently to the eggs hatching.1 In addition, fish created in isolation quickly form schools when placed in the company of other fish28 and show preference for joining others similar to the ones with which they were raised, even if they have different phenotypes, just like in an imprinting phenomenon of social preference acquisition during early development.29 The shoaling behavior increases in threatening situations and in response to a pheromone released as a result of injury to epidermal cells.1,30–32 This shows that shoaling is relevant in predator avoidance. According to Rehnberg and Smith,33 in the presence of threat there is greater group cohesion, with agitated swimming or subject’s immobilization close to the substrate. These behaviors increase the individual’s survival probability since fish schools are more likely to detect predators than an isolated fish.34 The shoaling behavior seems to have reinforcing properties,29 reducing anxiety and stress in fish. This is evident in the effects of different drugs in behavioral tests. In shoaling tests, in which the distance between conspecifics are measured, zebrafish exposed to anxiolytic substances, like LSD18 and alcohol,35 had the distance between subjects altered. Alcohol is a stimulating substance in lower doses and an inhibitor in higher doses. As expected, alcohol reduced the distance between subjects in lower doses and increased in higher doses.35 LSD was proven to enhance inter-fish distance in this test.18 Meanwhile, the alarm substance, a substance known for its anxiogenic and stressor effects, has reduced the distance between subjects.31 Altogether, these results suggest that shoaling behavior reduces anxiety and stress responses in zebrafish, since social cohesion is an indicator of anxiety.31 Computational simulations in laboratory have been successful in forecasting migratory movements in groups and schools movements based on temperature, salinity, and other ecological variables.36–38 According to Romey,39 the study of schools can be done in two ways. One way is to use models of

MANSUR ET AL.

population dynamics that investigate the size and the group’s displacement as a whole among the environment. Another way is to use behavioral models, which investigates the movement of individuals within the group. This study helps to elucidate how light variations play a vital role in computer algorithms/simulations that predict movement. The factors that contribute to the animal’s decision to initiate a movement include momentary contingencies and variables, such as hunger, exposure to predators and temperature, as well as durable and morphological variables, which include size, age, lipid levels, parasites, and genetic predisposition.40–46 The position of each individual in schools, seems to be due to the relative position of other group members, in an attraction and repulsion chain, in which an individual is attracted to conspecific members, unless they get too close, and if so, turning away.39 Behavioral models are used to gather information about behavior. Moreover, they need to better detect and point out the changes in brain and behavior. With the study of the light/ dark preference test applied to groups, we expect to verify the influence of conspecifics on the behavior of isolated individual group members. In the absence of significant change in behavior, group tests become an alternative to streamline and simplify data collection and test analysis. Objective

This research aims to describe and analyze the effects of a variable number of Danio rerio subjects, ranging from one to eight, on the light/dark preference test. To develop a more efficient data collection and to achieve an increased throughput test, we will also verify the feasibility of running the light/dark test with more than one subject. Materials and Methods Subjects

Four hundred eighty adult male short-finned phenotype zebrafish were acquired from a local pet shop and only experimentally naive fish were used in the experiments. They were kept in the laboratory for at least 2 weeks before the experiments, housed in two collective 30 L tanks (240 subjects per tank), measuring 32 · 34 · 67 cm, with controlled light exposure (12-h light–12-h dark, starting at 6:30 a.m.). The average pH was 6.8 and average temperature was held constant at 24C – 2C. The fish were fed once a day with chow (Color Tetra Tetra GmbH). The experiment was conducted in the Laboratory of Neuroscience and Behavior at the Center of Theory and Research of Behavior, Federal University of Para´ and the housing conditions met the APA Guidelines for Ethical Conduct in the Care and Use of Animals. The experimental procedures were approved by the ethics committee of UFPA (CEPAE: 138-13). Test apparatus and general procedure

All the experiments are conducted in rectangular acrylic opaque tanks (black/white boxes), half side black and half side white, each side measuring 15 · 10 · 45 cm, with water column of 10 cm height. The central compartment where the fish are placed measures 15 · 10 · 10 cm and it is delimited by sliding doors that can be pulled up. The sliding doors have the same color and material as the aquarium, one side is black

NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE?

3

and the other white. Two lamps illuminate the apparatuses, each of them cautiously positioned one meter above the extremities of the aquarium to keep a uniform illumination, avoiding shadow formation. After each session, the tanks are rotated 180 to prevent effects of spatial orientation (Fig. 1).

Table 1. Subjects Per Session Shows How Many Subjects Were Included in the Apparatus on Each Session; Subjects Per Group Shows the Total Number of Subjects on Each Group and Number of Sessions Shows the Number of Sessions for Each Group

Procedure

Subjects per session

The first subject is placed in the central compartment of the black/white box for a 5-min period (300 s) of habituation, to diminish effects of manipulation of the subjects. After this period the sliding doors are carefully removed at the same time and the subject can freely explore both compartments of the tank. The subject’s behavior is recorded with a video camera during 15 min (900 s). Afterward, the video files were analyzed and the occurrence of subjects on each side of the aquarium was registered every 15 s, resulting in 60 occurrences per session. Furthermore, the first side chosen by each subject was also recorded. There were four groups in this experiment and a different number of subjects was used in each group: the control group had only one subject inside the apparatus during the test, whereas the experimental groups had 2, 4, or 8 subjects placed simultaneously inside the apparatus on every session (As shown on Table 1). Each group was tested 32 times. To control day-to-day variations, each group had three sessions a day, totaling 12 sessions per day. All group types were tested each day. The tests were always performed between 8 and 11 a.m. and the subjects were not used in more than one test. No animals were hurt or sacrificed as a result of this experiment. Measurement methods

The total time spent by each subject on both sides of the tank is the main standard variable used to analyze dark/light preference. In this study the number of occurrence of subjects on each side of the aquarium was registered every 15 s. To validate this method, 16 sessions of the control group were analyzed using both techniques (total time and number of occurrences) for further comparison. Statistical analysis

To verify if the dark/light preference pattern changes with the number of subjects in the tank, the average occurrence

Ctrl 1 2 4 8

Subjects per group

Number of sessions

32 64 128 256

32 32 32 32

(AO) of subjects in each side of the aquarium and the first choice average was calculated and compared between groups. Since normality was not assumed for the data, the one-way analysis of variance Kruskal–Wallis test was used followed by Dunn’s post test. A Pearson correlation test was performed to determine if measuring the number of occurrences every 15 s is as accurate as measuring the total time of permanence. Results Validation of measurement methods

Preliminary descriptive statistics was run to compare the average total time spent by subjects in the white side of the apparatus and the average number of occurrences of subjects in the white side of the apparatus every 15 s, revealing similar results (as shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2). The Pearson correlation test comparing the two analysis methods was statistically significant, with a p-value of 0.000000000858. Furthermore, a very strong positive correlation coefficient r = 0.968 was found. Dark preference

After data collection and transcription it was possible to assess and compare the AO of subjects in the white side of the apparatus in each group. The Kruskal–Wallis test revealed significant variation between groups [H(3) = 18.838, p = 0.001] and the Dunn’s post test showed that the test group with eight subjects differs significantly from the control group and from the test group with two subjects (Fig. 3). A linear regression test was conducted to verify whether the AO of subjects has a linear relationship between groups. The linear regression test has shown a slightly positive linear relationship. R = 0.370; R2 = 0.137; Coefficient = 2.753; Standard Error = 0.615; t = 4.474; P = 0.001 (Fig. 4).

Table 2. Descriptive Statistics Comparing the Techniques of Registering Behavior (Total Time and Number of Occurrences)

FIG. 1.

Dark/light box setup.

Variable

n

Mean

Standard deviation

Standard error

Total time Number of occurrences

16 16

17,264 17,111

13,478 14,228

3369 3557

4

MANSUR ET AL.

FIG. 4. Linear regression line and data points of the average occurrence of subjects in the white side of the apparatus.

The data indicates no significant differences in the light/dark preference test when carried out with as many as four subjects,

meanwhile, the test group with eight subjects showed statistical differences, when compared with the control group and the test group with two subjects. This indicates that the light/ dark test may be done with up to four subjects in the same tank, during the same session, without affecting the light/dark preference behavior of individual subjects. The white side of the apparatus is an aversive stimulus to the fish. Given the significant results obtained with the eight subjects group, it is possible to deduce that the shoaling behavior reduces stress and anxiety.31 In the light/dark box experiment with more than one subject, there are some competing stimuli that induce the animal to move to the white side or to the dark side. The animal’s natural tendency to seek protection in dark places, avoiding bright environments, and the anxiety caused by new environments contribute to the preference for dark places. On the other hand, the innate drive to explore new places added to a possible anxiolytic effect caused by the presence of other subjects inside the apparatus contributed to the subject’s displacement to the white side. Zebrafish are a highly social animal, exhibiting preference for swimming in groups and many other patterns of social cohesion. The reduced white avoidance behavior in the group of eight subjects reflects the role of shoaling, which is a defense

FIG. 3. Average occurrence (plus standard error) of subjects in the white side of the apparatus measured on every 15 s. **Differs from control and from group 2, assuming that p < 0.05. The average occurrence is calculated for each group adding up the percentage of occurrence of subjects in the white side of all sessions (32 per group) and then dividing the result by the number of sessions (32).

FIG. 5. Average of subjects (plus standard error) that have first chosen the white side of the apparatus. *Differs from control, assuming that p < 0.05. The average occurrence is calculated for each group adding up the subjects that have first chosen the white side within the 32 sessions and then dividing this number by the number of sessions.

FIG. 2. Descriptive statistics graphic comparing the average percentages of total time spent in the white side and average occurrence of subjects in the white side on every 15 s. First choice

It was also assessed and compared the average of subjects that have first chosen the white side of the apparatus within each group. The Kruskal–Wallis test revealed significant variation between groups [H(3) = 10.167, p = 0.017] and the Dunn’s post test showed difference between control and the group with eight subjects (Fig. 5). A linear regression test was conducted to verify if the average of subjects that first chose the white side of the apparatus has a linear relationship between groups. The linear regression test did not show any positive or negative relationship between these variables. R = 0.0416; R2 = 0.00173; Coefficient = 0.364; Standard Error = 0.779; t = 0.468; P = 0.641 (Fig. 6). Discussion

NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE?

FIG. 6. Linear regression line and data points of the average of subjects that have first chosen the white side of the apparatus. The numbers beside the dots represent frequency of the events. mechanism, in reducing anxiety and stress. However, the test results show that four subjects inside the apparatus is not enough to cause the anxiolytic effects of shoaling in the light/ dark test. Figure 4 shows that the regression line has a mild positive tendency for the AO of subjects in the white side of the apparatus; it might reflect a slight decrease in white avoidance even in the group of four subjects. In the novel tank test, for example, four subjects are enough to form a shoal with consistent performance in the test.47 Perhaps only four subjects in a school is not enough to elicit all the benefits observed in larger schools, such as food location, efficient displacement, food acquisition, reproductive improvement and, particularly, predator detection and avoidance. It reflects the subject’s performance on the test, since there is no significant reduction in white avoidance behavior in the groups with two and four subjects. These findings support future test with more than one subject, since shoal cohesion remains stable and practically unaltered and only stimuli of considerable magnitude are able to alter its natural condition.48 Therefore, the subjects are prone to behave accordingly to the scototaxis behavior. The variables commonly measured in the light/dark box test are the total time in each side of the box; number of midline crossings; latency for the first choice; first choice of compartment and thigmotaxis. The measurement of the time of permanence is related to dark preference/white avoidance behavior and can be used as a measure of anxiety, whereas thigmotaxis and first choice are measures of locomotor behavior and fear.6,8 From the above mentioned variables, this research tested only for first choice behavior. The total time of permanence of subjects in each side of the apparatus would be a very difficult task to perform, since there were too many subjects being observed at the same moment. Therefore, instead of the total time of permanence of each subject in each side of the apparatus, this research measured the occurrence of subjects in each side of the apparatus every 15 s. This measure has proven to be efficient and just as reliable as the measurement of total time (as shown in Table 2 and Fig. 2). Accordingly to this, the present model with enhanced numbers of subjects has the potential to provide faster outcomes and new possibilities for the light/dark box test. However, researchers are advised to consider the limitations of this method. Although faster and more convenient, in this varia-

5

tion of the test, it would be difficult to measure the total time of permanence and thigmotaxis. The results of the research may also have been influenced by two outside variables, social behavior and/or shoaling, and the preference for dark places. Social behavior is presented by the Danio rerio species, which has an established hierarchy in the shoal, where the subordinated subjects follow the dominant subjects. In this study, fish of Danio rerio species were placed to swim in a 15 · 10 · 45 cm dimensions light/dark box with a 10 cm water column, containing 4.5 L. According to Aoki,49 there is a regular distance between individuals in a shoal, which rises due to the maintenance of a private space for each individual. Our tests were made successfully with up to four subjects in the same apparatus. The inefficacy in the test with eight subjects was possibly due to the lack of space inside the test aquarium that lead the researchers to investigate whether the scototaxic behavior is altered or supplanted by a tendency to maintain the distance between subjects. According to Westerfield,50 zebrafish is a shoaling fish and can be maintained at high concentrations such as 20 subjects per 3 L of water at 3 months of age. In our experiment the highest concentration of fish was eight subjects per 6.75 m3 of water. However, studies using light/dark boxes of different sizes and with different water volumes should be further conducted. The test still needs to undergo pharmacological validation to verify the effectiveness of utilizing more than one subject in the presence of drugs, provided that some drugs can modify both fish and shoal behavior,47,51 resulting in cognitive impairments, lowering or altering social behavior. It is possible that such deficit in the subject’s ability to follow conspecifics may harm the group’s movement cohesion, causing the absence of shoaling behavior. Nevertheless, even without this kind of behavior, it is likely that the results in the light/dark test remain unmodified in the presence of any drugs, if the fish scototaxis behavior remains intact, because the white avoidance behavior would most likely keep the subjects in the dark side. This study does not attempt to determine if the results are due to social behavior or scototaxis, therefore, further studies with anxiolytic drugs are necessary to investigate this matter. Conclusion

The reduced white avoidance behavior in the group of eight subjects seems to reflect the role of shoaling, which is a defense mechanism in reducing anxiety and stress. On the other hand, the absence of significant difference between the control group and the test groups with two and four subjects suggest feasibility to run the light/dark test with up to four subjects, becoming an alternative to streamline and to simplify the data collection and test analysis. Pharmacological tests are still required to determine whether the effect of drugs alters the model with enhanced number of subjects. Acknowledgments

This research was supported by grants from CAPES (Coordination for the Improvement of Higher Level or Education Personnel). Disclosure Statement

No competing financial interests exist.

6

MANSUR ET AL.

References

1. Engeszer RE, Patterson LB, Rao AA, Parichy DM. Zebrafish in the wild: a review of natural history and new notes from the field. Zebrafish 2007;4:21–40. 2. Paull G, Filby A, Giddins H, Coe T, Hamilton P, Tyler C. Dominance hierarchies in zebrafish (Danio rerio) and their relationship with reproductive success. Zebrafish 2010;7: 109–117. 3. Houart C. Zebrafish as an Experimental Organism. In: eLS. John Wiley & Sons Ltd, Chichester. http://www.els.net [doi: 10.1038/npg.els.0002094]. 4. Spence R, Gerlach G, Lawrence C, Smith C. The behaviour and ecology of the zebrafish, Danio rerio. Biol Rev Camb Philos Soc 2008;83:13–34. 5. Spence R, Fatema MK, Reichard M, Huq KA., Wahab MA., Ahmed ZF, et al. The distribution and habitat preferences of the zebrafish in Bangladesh. J Fish Biol 2006; 69:1435–1448. 6. Maximino C, Brito TM, Dias CAGDM, Gouveia A Jr., Morato S. Scototaxis as anxiety-like behavior in fish. Nat Protoc 2010;5:209–216. 7. Gouveia A Jr., Zampieri RA, Ramos LA, Silva EF, Mattioli R, Morato S. Preference of Goldfish (carassius auratus) for dark places. Rev Etol 2005;7:63–66. 8. Maximino C, Brito TM, Moraes FD, Oliveira FVC, Taccolini IB, Pereira PM, et al. A comparative analysis of the preference for dark environments in five teleosts. Int J Comp Psychol 2007;20:351–367. 9. Blaser RE, Chadwick L, McGinnis GC. Behavioral measures of anxiety in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2010;208:56–62. 10. Egan RJ, Bergner CL, Hart PC, Cachat JM, Canavello PR, Elegante MF, et al. Understanding behavioral and physiological phenotypes of stress and anxiety in zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 2009;205:38–44. 11. Wong K, Elegante M, Bartels B, Elkhayat S, Tien D, Roy S, et al. Analyzing habituation responses to novelty in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2010;208:450–457. 12. McNaughton N, Corr PJ. A two-dimensional neuropsychology of defense: fear/anxiety and defensive distance. Neurosci Biobehav Rev 2004;28:285–305. 13. Blanchard DC, Blanchard RJ. Ethoexperimental approaches to the biology of emotion. Ann Rev Psychol 1988;39:43–68. 14. Catherall DR. How fear differs from anxiety. Traumatology 2003;9:76–92. 15. Godsil BP, Tinsley MR, Fanselow MS: Motivation. In: Handbook of Psychology: Experimental Psychology. Healy AF and Proctor RW (eds), pp. 33–60, John Wiley & Sons, Hoboken, NJ, 2003,. 16. Bolles R, Beecher M: Behavioristic approach to aversively a functional motivated behavior: predatory imminence as a determinant of the topography of defensive behavior. In: Evolution and Learning. Bolles RC and Beecher MD (eds), pp. 185–211, Erlbaum, Hillsdale, MI, 2013. 17. Maximino C, Brito TM, da Silva Batista AW, Herculano AM, Morato S, Gouveia A Jr. Measuring anxiety in zebrafish: a critical review. Behav Brain Res 2010;214:157–171. 18. Grossman L, Utterback E, Stewart A, Gaikwad S, Chung KM, Suciu C, et al. Characterization of behavioral and endocrine effects of LSD on zebrafish. Behav Brain Res 2010;214:277–284. 19. Maximino C, da Silva AWB, Gouveia A, Herculano AM. Pharmacological analysis of zebrafish (Danio rerio) scoto-

20.

21. 22. 23. 24. 25.

26.

27. 28. 29. 30. 31. 32. 33.

34. 35. 36. 37. 38.

39.

taxis. Prog Neuropsychopharmacol Biol Psychiatry 2011; 35:624–631. Maximino C, Brito TM, Colmanetti R, Pontes AAA, Castro HM, Lacerda RI, Morato S, et al. Parametric analyses of anxiety in zebrafish scototaxis. Behav Brain Res 2010; 210:1–7. Mansur BDM, Rodrigues B, Gouveia A Jr. Efeitos da substaˆncia de alarme no teste claro/escuro no Zebrafish. Biota Amaz 2014;4:87–93. Jesuthasan S. Fear, anxiety, and control in the zebrafish. Dev Neurobiol 2012;72:395–403. Wang J, Liu C, Ma F, Chen W, Liu J, Hu B, et al. Circadian clock mediates light/dark preference in zebrafish (danio rerio). Zebrafish 2014;11:2014. Saverino C, Gerlai R. The social zebrafish: behavioral responses to conspecific, heterospecific, and computer animated fish. Behav Brain Res 2008;191:77–87. Pham M, Raymond J, Hester J, Kyzar E, Gaikwad S, Bruce I, et al. Assessing social behavior phenotypes in adult zebrafish: shoaling, social preference and mirror biting tests. Zebrafish Protoc Neurobehav Res 2012; 1–27. Romey WL, Galbraith E. Optimal group positioning after a predator attack: the influence of speed, sex, and satiation within mobile whirligig swarms. Behav Ecol 2007;19: 338–343. Engeszer RE, Ryan MJ, Parichy DM. Learned social preference in zebrafish. Curr Biol 2004;14:881–884. Kerr J. Grouping behavior of the zebrafish as influenced by social isolation. Am Zool 1963;2:532–533. Al-Imari L, Gerlai R. Sight of conspecifics as reward in associative learning in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2008;189:216–219. Wisenden BD, Vollbrecht KA, Brown JL. Is there a fish alarm cue? Affirming evidence from a wild study. Anim Behav 2004;67:59–67. Speedie N, Gerlai R. Alarm substance induced behavioral responses in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2008;188:168–177. Pfeiffer W: Pheromones in fish and amphibia. In: Pheromones. Birch M (ed), vol. 32, pp. 269–296, North-Holland, Amsterdam, 1974. Rehnberg BG, Smith RJF. The influence of alarm substance and shoal size on the behaviour of zebra danios, Brachydanio rerio (Cyprinidae). J Fish Biol 1988;33: 155–163. Pitcher T, Parrish J: Functions of shoaling in teleosts Behaviour. In: Behaviour of Teleost Fishes. Pitcher T (ed), pp. 363–439, Chapman and Hall, London, 1993. Kurta A, Palestis BG. Effects of ethanol on the shoaling behavior of zebrafish (danio rerio). Dose Response 2010;8:527–533. Anderson JJAY. A stochastic model for the size of fish schools 1. Fish Bull 1981;79:315–323. Reyes E, Sklar FH, Day JW. A regional organism exchange model for simulating fish migration. Ecol Model 1994; 74:255–276. Peter Nonacs, Paul E. Smith, Amos Bouskila BL. Modeling the behavior of the northern anchovy, Engraulis mordax, as a shooling predator exploiting patchy prey. Deep Sea Research 1994;41:147–169. Romey WL. Individual differences make a difference in the trajectories of simulated schools of fish. Ecol Model 1996;92:65–77.

NUMBER OF SUBJECTS ALTERS DARK/LIGHT PREFERENCE?

40. Theodorakis CW. Size segregation and the effects of oddity on predation risk in minnow schools. Anim Behav 1989; 38:496–502. 41. Wellington W. Individual differences as a factor in population dynamics: the development of a problem. Can J Zool 1957;35:293–323. 42. Slater P: Individual differences in animal behavior. In: Perspectives in Ethology. Bateson P and Klopfer P (ed), pp. 35–49, Plenum Press, New York, 1981. 43. Clark A, Ehlinger T: Pattern and adaptation in individual behavioral differences. In: Perspectives in Ethology. Bateson P and Klopfer P (ed), pp. 1–47, Plenum Press, New York, 1987. 44. Colgan P: The motivational basis of fish behaviour. In: Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edition. Pitcher T (ed), pp. 23–43, Chapman and Hall, New York, 1993. 45. Magurran A: Individual differences in fish behaviour. In: Behaviour of Teleost Fishes, 2nd edition. Pitcher T (ed), pp. 441–477, Chapman and Hall, New York, 1986. 46. Romey W, Rossman D. Temperature and hunger alter grouping trade-offs in whirligig beetles. Am Midl Nat 1995;134:51–62. 47. Green J, Collins C, Kyzar EJ, Pham M, Roth A, Gaikwad S, et al. Automated high-throughput neurophenotyping of zebrafish social behavior. J Neurosci Methods 2012;210: 266–271.

7

48. Miller N, Gerlai R. Quantification of shoaling behaviour in zebrafish (Danio rerio). Behav Brain Res 2007;184: 157–166. 49. Aoki I. A simulation study on the schoolng mechanism in fish. Bull Jpn Soc Sci Fish 1982;48:1081–1088. 50. Westerfield M: The Zebrafish Book: A Guide for the Laboratory Use of Zebrafish (Danio rerio), 4th edition. University of Oregon Press, Eugene, 2000. 51. Maaswinkel H, Le X, He L, Zhu L, Weng W. Dissociating the effects of habituation, black walls, buspirone and ethanol on anxiety-like behavioral responses in shoaling zebrafish. A 3D approach to social behavior. Pharmacol Biochem Behav 2013;108:16–27.

Address correspondence to: Bruno de Matos Mansur, BS Universidade Federal do Para´ Rua Augusto Correˆa 01-Guama´ CEP 66075-110 Caixa postal 479 Bele´m 2012 Para´ Brasil E-mail: [email protected]

light preference of Zebrafish (Danio rerio).

This research aims to describe the effects of a variable number of Danio rerio fish subjects, ranging from one to eight, in the light/dark box prefere...
249KB Sizes 0 Downloads 6 Views