Gestational Age and Developmental Risk in Moderately and Late Preterm and Early Term Infants Luisa Schonhaut, MD, MPH, MSca,b, Iván Armijo, BScc, Marcela Pérez, MDa,b

OBJECTIVES:

The objective of this study was to evaluate the association between gestational age (GA) at birth and risk of developmental delay at 8 and 18 months of corrected postnatal age.

abstract

METHODS: During

2008 to 2011, infants at a corrected postnatal age of 8 or 18 months attending health centers in Santiago, Chile, were recruited. Participants completed a form on biographical and demographic characteristics and the Chilean validated version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition (ASQ). Logistic regression was used to detect the capacity of GA to predict scores , 22 SDs on the basis of the Chilean ASQ reference group, in at least 1 ASQ domain, adjusted by different control variables. A total of 1667 infants were included in the analysis. An inverse “dose response” relationship between developmental delay risk and GA at birth was found, both in the crude and adjusted models. Compared with those born full term, the odds ratio for developmental delay risk was 1.56 for those born early term (95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.19–2.06), 2.58 for infants born late preterm (95%CI: 1.66–4.01), and 3.01 for those born moderately preterm (95%CI: 1.59–5.71).

RESULTS:

CONCLUSIONS: An inverse dose-response relationship between GA and risk of developmental delay

was found in the tested population. Future prospective studies and predictive models are needed to understand whether this higher developmental risk in moderately and late preterm infants is transient and modifiable or persists throughout life, allowing for better targeting of early-intervention strategies.

a Departmento de Pediatria, Clínica Alemana, Santiago, Chile; bFacultad de Medicina, Clinica Alemana-Universidad del Desarrollo, Santiago, Chile; and cFacultad de Psicología, Universidad Gabriela Mistral, Santiago, Chile

Dr Schonhaut conceptualized and designed this study, coordinated and supervised data collection, and drafted the initial manuscript; Mr Armijo carried out the statistical analyses and reviewed and revised the manuscript; Dr Pérez recruited the patients and reviewed and revised the manuscript; and all authors approved the final manuscript as submitted. www.pediatrics.org/cgi/doi/10.1542/peds.2014-1957 DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1957 Accepted for publication Jan 27, 2015 Address correspondence to Luisa Schonhaut, MD, MPH, MSc, Departamento de Pediatria, Clínica Alemana, Vitacura 5951, Santiago, Chile. E-mail: [email protected] PEDIATRICS (ISSN Numbers: Print, 0031-4005; Online, 1098-4275). Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics

WHAT’S KNOWN ON THIS SUBJECT: There is growing evidence reporting that moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term infants are at increased risk of developmental delay. The characteristics of this association are not well established in the literature. WHAT THIS STUDY ADDS: In a sample of infants born between 32 and 41 weeks, there was an inverse and “dose response” relationship between gestational age and developmental delay risk using the ASQ at 8 and 18 months of corrected postnatal age.

FINANCIAL DISCLOSURE: The authors have indicated they have no financial relationships relevant to this article to disclose.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015

ARTICLE

Over the past decades, developed countries have seen a decrease in their natality rates, with a relative increase in births before 40 weeks of gestational age (GA).1,2 This same trend has occurred in Chile, where total preterm births increased from 5.0% to 6.6% over the past 17 years.3 This situation can be explained by an increased obstetric risk as a result of late primiparity, an increase in multiple pregnancies, and better pregnancy and labor monitoring techniques.4 There has been growing research regarding the risk of moderately preterm (MPT; born by 320/7 to 336/7 weeks of gestation), late preterm (LPT; 340/7 to 366/7 weeks), and early term (ET; 370/7 to 386/7 weeks) children compared with full-term (FT) infants (390/7 to 416/7 weeks of gestation) since 2005. These children have a higher risk of short-term perinatal morbidity2,5–7 and longterm developmental, behavioral, and learning difficulties.8,9–18 However, despite the evidence, there is still controversy regarding the actual impact of delivery before the end of gestation. For example, there is debate about the use of corrected postnatal age at the time of assessment of MPT and LPT infants. Woythaler et al19 showed that LPT infants had a 50% increased risk of developmental delay at a chronological age of 2 years. Romeo et al20 compared the performance of LPT children at 12 and 18 months, with and without correcting for postnatal age, and found that differences compared with children born FT disappeared after correction. On the other hand, Walsh et al21 recently showed that a group of MPT and LPT infants had smaller brain size, less myelination, and more immature gyral folding measured by MRI compared with term infants at 38 to 44 weeks’ corrected GA. Another controversy focuses on the determinants of developmental impact. Some authors have

2

speculated that it could be the result of prematurity, whereas others believe that it is due to associated factors, such as morbidity and NICU admission.22 Gurka et al23 reported that healthy LPT infants born after 340/7 weeks, without neonatal complications, had long-term cognition, achievement, socioemotional, and behavioral development equivalent to FT infants. Baron et al24 found lower performance in infants with complicated LPT birth compared with those with uncomplicated birth. This hypothesis has been discredited by MacGowan et al,25 who found no significant differences in early childhood development between LPT infants admitted to the NICU and those who were not. A real difficulty in drawing conclusions regarding the impact of delivery before term is the heterogeneity of instruments used and diverse ages of infants at the time of assessments, as well as the fact that most published studies were conducted in retrospective or population-based cohorts.11,26 The objective of this study was to evaluate the association between GA at birth and risk of developmental delay at 8 and 18 months of corrected postnatal age by using the Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition (ASQ).27 These ages were selected on the basis of Chilean public health guidelines for screening of development in children.28

METHODS A cross-sectional study was carried out with the use of a convenience sample of infants aged 8 to 18 months. The ET and FT sample of infants was extracted from the normative sample who participated in the validation study of the ASQ for the Chilean population, which took place between May 2008 and November 2011. In the same time period, MPT and LPT infants were selected by direct contact in health services. Children were

recruited from public health centers located in the southeastern and northern districts and from a private eastern center of the metropolitan region of Santiago, Chile.

Participants A convenience sample was constructed on the basis of patients who attended the health center for their routine follow-up visit. Term infants were recruited at scheduled health visits, and preterm infants were identified on the basis of local birth databases in each center and were contacted purposely for the study. Parents or primary caregivers who agreed to participate signed an informed consent and completed a form with medical and biographical and demographic information. GA information was disclosed by the parents and verified with medical records. The chronological age of infants born at #366/7 weeks was corrected by subtracting the weeks remaining to complete 400/7 weeks. Infants were classified on the basis of GA as MPT (320/7 to 336/7 weeks of gestation), LPT (340/7 to 366/7 weeks of gestation), ET (370/7 to 386/7 weeks of gestation), or FT (390/7 to 416/7 weeks of gestation).26 Infants aged 7 to 8 months and 30 days and 17 to 18 months and 30 days were included in the study. Each child was tested only once during the study either at 8 or 18 months. The tests were completed on the basis of the appropriated chronological (for FT and ET) or corrected postnatal (for MPT and LPT) ages. Infants with known genetic, metabolic, or neurologic disease (n = 14); those born at ,32 weeks (n = 10); or those born equal at $420/7 weeks (n = 33) were excluded. Those infants without known GA (n = 54) were also excluded. After exclusions, a final sample of 1667 patients was included in the analysis.

Outcome Measures The ASQ is a brief measure in which parents rate their child’s current

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

SCHONHAUT et al

skills and development from 1 to 66 months of age. Twenty-one questionnaires are available within this age range. Parents answer 30 questions covering 5 domains of development, including communication, gross motor, fine motor, problem-solving, and adaptive skills. Parents are instructed to try activities with their child to facilitate accurate assessment. A pass/fail score was assigned for each area of development. The presence of any domain that screened , 22 SDs below the mean area score for the Chilean reference group was considered to be a positive screen, indicating risk of developmental delay. The ASQ has been validated in several countries.29,30,31 In Chile, the sensitivity and specificity ranges of 75% (95% CI 62%–85%) and 81% (95% CI 76%–86%), respectively, have been reported.32 Parents also completed a form with personal, socioeconomic, demographic, perinatal, and medical information.

Ethics The ethics committees of the National Fund approved this study for Health Research (FONIS), Southwest Santiago Health Service, Southeast Santiago Health Service, and Facultad de Medicina Clinica AlemanaUniversidad del Desarrollo.

admission, small for GA (birth weight ,10th percentile according to validated tables for Chilean population),34 gender, multiple birth, and ASQ administration age (8 or 18 month). Analyses were performed by using R (Vienna, Austria) and Stata 12 (StataCorp, College Station, TX) platform statistics with a linear trend module.

RESULTS Perinatal and biographical and demographic characteristics of the 1667 participants are shown in Table 1. We observed that the GA distribution of infants evaluated at 8 and 18 months was homogeneous. There was a comparable frequency of teenage pregnancy in all GA groups. In contrast, there was a higher incidence of small for GA, NICU admitted, and multiple births in the preterm infants groups compared with FT group. A trend toward decreased risk of developmental delay as GA increased was observed (42.86%, 37.58%, and 23.81% for MPT, LPT, and ET, respectively, compared with 17.63% for FT children). An inverse “dose response” relationship between GA

and risk of developmental delay was also observed (Fig 1). When adjusting for predefined variables, the odds of developmental delay risk based on ASQ scores also increased significantly and were inversely related to GA. Compared with those born FT, odds ratios (ORs) were 1.56 (95% confidence interval [CI]: 1.19–2.06) for those born ET, 2.58 (95% CI: 1.66–4.01) for infants born LPT, and 3.01 (95% CI: 1.59–5.71) for those born MPT. The number of affected domains increased as GA decreased. In MPT infants the OR was significant for all domains except for communication. In LPT infants the OR was significant for gross and fine motor domains, whereas for ET infants the OR was significant only in gross motor skills (Table 2). There was a trend for linearity between adjusted risk of developmental delay and GA in the 33- to 40-week interval (Fig 2). The x2 for trend of ORs was 27.33 (P , .001). An evaluation for adjustment of linear trend of the relation between GA and development delay risk was significant (F = 81.87, P , .001, R2 = 0.90) and no significant improvements in adjustment were found for quadratic (F = 3.19, P = .11)

TABLE 1 Background Characteristics of Participating Children n

Statistical Analysis Sample characteristics were described by using the risk frequency for risk of developmental delay in the different GA groups. With the use of data on this criterion, a multivariateadjusted logistic regression model was constructed. In addition, a series of dummy variables were generated to identify the risk associated with each week of gestation; 40 weeks of GA was used as a reference. The analysis included the following control variables: socioeconomic status (based on the socioeconomic classification used as a reference for our country),33 teenage mother (,19 years old at the time of birth), NICU

GA Group 07

07

P 07

MPT (32 to LPT (34 to ET (37 to FT(39 to 336/7 Weeks) 366/7 Weeks) 386/7 Weeks) 416/7 Weeks) ASQ form, n (%) 8 mo 18 mo Total Birth weight, mean (SD), g Male gender, n (%) Socioeconomic status: medium-low (vs medium-high), n (%) Adolescent mother, n (%) NICU admission, n (%) Multiple pregnancies, n (%) Small for GA, n (%)

.799 864 803 1667 1655

42 35 77 1879

(4.9) (4.4) (4.6) (504)

87 78 165 2532

(10.1) (9.7) (9.9) (548)

274(31.7) 272 (33.9) 546 (32.7) 3188 (414)

461 418 879 3488

(53.4) (52.1) (52.8) (400)

,.001

1667 811 (48.7) 1667 1067 (64.0)

48 (62.3)

89 (53.9)

258 (47.3)

416 (47.3)

.034

43 (55.8)

70 (42.4)

363 (66.5)

591 (67.2)

,.001

1667 242 (14.5) 1667 279 (16.7) 1667 67 (5.8) 1655 232 (14.0)

9 (11.7)

15 (9.1)

81 (14.8)

137 (15.6)

.154

72 (93.5)

87 (52.7)

59 (10.8)

61 (6.9)

,.001

36 (46.8)

46 (27.9)

11 (2.0)

4 (0.5)

,.001

30 (39.0)

50 (30.3)

64 (11.9)

88 (10.1)

,.001

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015

07

3

FIGURE 1 Rate of developmental delay risk based on the ASQ by GA. Bars represent weeks of gestation; lines represent 95% CIs of developmental delay risk for every week; and colors represent GA groups. Post hoc pairwise comparisons using Tukey’s honestly significant difference test were conducted between MPT, LPT, ET, and FT infants: *P , .05, **P , .01, ***P , .001.

or cubic (F = 0.16, P = .70) fit, when compared with linear fit. When evaluating the effect of each factor isolated from others using univariate analysis, we found associations between risk of developmental delay and GA, NICU admission, multiple pregnancy, and male gender. When controlling for the effects of other predictors using multivariate analysis, GA, male gender, adolescent mother, and ASQ administration age remained significant (Table 3).

DISCUSSION We found an inverse, dose-response relationship between corrected GA and risk of developmental delay considering birth at 40 weeks as a reference and after adjusting for predefined control variables. In accordance with our results, previous studies that used the ASQ to identify children at risk of developmental delay found greater developmental delay risk in LPT and MPT infants compared with term infants.8,35

TABLE 2 Crude and Adjusted ORs and 95% CIs for Developmental Delay Based on the ASQ MPT (n = 77)

Total Crude Adjusted Communication Crude Adjusted Gross motor Crude Adjusted Fine motor Crude Adjusted Problem-solving Crude Adjusted Personal-social Crude Adjusted

LPT (n = 165)

ET (n = 546)

OR

OR

95% CI

95% CI

FT (n = 879) x 2 (df = 3)

P

OR

95% CI

OR

3.50 3.01

2.15–5.66 1.59–5.71

2.81 1.95–4.02 1.46 1.13–1.90 2.58 1.66–4.01 1.56 1.19–2.06

1.00 1.00

55.35 26.02

,.01 ,.01

5.39 1.50

2.01–13.08 4.23 1.92–9.07 1.11 0.50–2.39 0.39–5.48 1.53 0.51–4.24 1.05 0.47–2.27

1.00 1.00

30.5 0.41

,.01 .94

2.49 2.64

1.28–4.54 1.16–5.88

2.91 1.84–4.52 1.60 1.12–2.27 3.37 1.92–5.86 1.74 1.20–2.52

1.00 1.00

27,04 17,47

,.01 ,.01

2.69 4.28

1.38–4.93 1.83–9.82

1.86 1.09–3.07 1.17 0.79–1.72 2.55 1.39–4.56 1.24 0.83–1.84

1.00 1.00

13,70 14.23

,.01 ,.01

4.88 2.93

2.25–9.96 1.03–8.04

2.50 1.23–4.83 1.53 0.89–2.61 1.94 0.83–4.26 1.48 0.85–2.57

1.00 1.00

23.87 6.70

,.01 .087

4.89 3.31

2.39–9.51 1.23–8.77

3.32 1.84–5.86 1.35 0.81–2.24 2.00 0.9–4.95 1.49 0.88–2.51

1.00 1.00

33.23 6.26

,.01 .09

The analysis included the following control variables: socioeconomic status, teenage mother (,19 years old at the time of birth), NICU admission, small for gestational age (birth weight ,10th percentile according to validated tables for the Chilean population), gender, multiple birth, and ASQ form. df, degrees of freedom.

4

In our study, there was a significant linear inverse relationship between weeks of GA and risk of developmental delay. In addition, no significant improvements were found comparing that adjustment with quadratic or cubic fit. Studies that included extremely preterm children found a linear association between decreasing GA and cognitive development,36,37 whereas others reported an exponential association below 36 weeks by using the ASQ.37 Nevertheless, all studies reported a consistent inverse relationship.13,39–42 The inverse relationship between weeks of GA and frequency of psychomotor problems has a physiologic explanation. It has been described that the last weeks of pregnancy are a critical period for fetal brain development at molecular, neurochemical, and structural levels.43,44 Kugelman and Colin45 presented a descriptive model that combined the pathology that is responsible for preterm delivery and the exposure of the immature brain to the hazards present in the extrauterine environment. Kerstjens et al22 described that LPT infants exposed to neonatal morbidities presented the highest risk of developmental delay. Moreover, the recent data published by Walsh et al21 demonstrated that there is a significant measurable difference in brain size, myelination, and gyral folding between MPT, LPT, and term newborns after correcting for GA at birth, which could potentially explain the differences in development. In our study, we found high frequencies of risk of developmental delay over all GA groups, comparable to other researchers who used the ASQ.30,46 Those frequencies were higher for 8-month-old children than for 18-month-old children. In the multivariate analysis, the most significant factor for developmental delay risk was ASQ administration age. Other studies conducted by our

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

SCHONHAUT et al

considering the application guidelines for the ASQ, with a 2-month window for child evaluation, correcting 2 or 3 weeks of age for ET has never changed the recommended application form.

FIGURE 2 Trend of ORs for risk of developmental delay based on the ASQ using 40 weeks of GA as the baseline. Note: ORs were adjusted to control the effects of ASQ administration age (8 or 18 month) socioeconomic status, teenage mother, NICU admission, small for gestational age, gender, and multiple birth. Higher log ORs indicate higher probability of the presence of developmental delay risk in the child (equal OR: x 2 = 54.69, P , .001; trend of odds: x 2 = 27.33, P , .001). Solid line shows trend of ORs for risk of developmental delay and shaded area shows 95 CI trend.

TABLE 3 Univariate and Multivariate Analyses for Risk of Developmental Delay Based on the ASQ Variable

Univariate Analysis OR (95% CI)

Fetal and neonatal risk factors Weeks remaining to 40 weeks of GAa NICU admission Small for GA Multiple pregnancy Male gender Other factors Low socioeconomic status Adolescent mother ASQ administration age a

Multivariate Analysis P

OR (95% CI)

P

(1.15–1.28) (1.41–2.48) (0.79–1.51) (1.73–4.03) (1.11–1.75)

,.001 ,.001 .573 ,.001 .004

1.21 0.96 0.85 1.35 1.42

(1.12–1.31) (0.65–1.40) (0.58–1.22) (0.78–2.28) (1.11–1.81)

,.001 .832 .383 .268 .005

0.64 (0.51–0.81) 0.53 (0.36–0.76) 3.15 (2.46–4.06)

,.001 ,.001 ,.001

0.88 (0.68–1.15) 0.56 (0.37–0.84) 3.36 (2.60–4.38)

.342 .006 ,.001

1.21 1.87 1.10 2.65 1.39

Weeks remaining to 40 weeks of GA is included as a continuous variable and ranges from 0 to 8.

group also found a higher frequency of lower ASQ scores in smaller infants, probably associated with parenting practices, with a late catchup in development. Another significant factor was male gender, which is in accordance with the results of other authors, although there is no a clear explanation of the reason for this difference39,47 Some authors even proposed the use of differentiated norms for boys and girls, but this issue still needs more research.29 Unexpectedly, giving birth in adolescence appears to be a protective factor for the risk of developmental delay. Although there

is evidence suggesting that teen mothers could be less accurate in identifying developmental delay risk in their children,48 we believe that these results require further research, including the impact of cultural factors that could affect parenting styles, such as family constitution and family network support for the adolescent mother. We found it appropriate to correct the GA of MPT and LPT infants both at 8 and 18 months of age on the basis of previous reports.20 Although currently there are no published data to support correcting GA in ET infants, we believe this is a topic that needs further research. Nevertheless,

A limitation of this study could be its cross-sectional design. Although the response rate was high, there is no information on the number of eligible children who declined to participate. Because of the nature of the sampling procedure (convenience sampling), the sample is not necessarily representative of the overall population and some GA groups could be over- or underrepresented. It is possible that there was a greater representation of parents/caregivers who were concerned about the health and development of their children, a variable that could be potentially relevant as has been proposed by other researchers.49 To increase the knowledge on these associations and to clarify the controversies raised, it is critical to have better prospective studies and predictive models to understand the long-term effect of each developmental risk factor in these populations. Prospective research is necessary for a better understanding of whether the impact of moderate and late prematurity is transient and potentially modifiable or if the differences persist throughout life.50,51 Clearly, much research remains to be done in these preterm populations to understand how these differences in development evolve throughout childhood and for better targeting of early-intervention strategies.

ACKNOWLEDGMENTS We thank Dr Xavier Demestre and Dr Kevin Marks for reviewing and editorial assistance. We also thank Dr Sergio Muñoz for statistical assistance and Dr Andres Maturana for final review and amendment of the final version of the manuscript.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015

5

FUNDING: Supported by the Clinica Alemana research grants program and grant SA7I20043 from the National Fund for Health Research (FONIS). POTENTIAL CONFLICT OF INTEREST: The authors have indicated they have no potential conflicts of interest to disclose.

REFERENCES 1. Blencowe H, Cousens S, Oestergaard MZ, et al. National, regional, and worldwide estimates of preterm birth rates in the year 2010 with time trends since 1990 for selected countries: a systematic analysis and implications. Lancet. 2012; 379(9832):2162–2172 2. Guasch XD, Torrent FR, Martínez-Nadal S, Cerén CV, Saco MJ, Castellví PS. Prematuros tardíos: una población de riesgo infravalorada [Late preterm infants: a population at underestimated risk]. An Pediatr (Barc). 2009;71(4):291–298 3. López PO, Bréart G. Trends in gestational age and birth weight in Chile, 1991-2008: a descriptive epidemiological study. BMC Pregnancy Childbirth. 2012;12:121 4. Spong CY, Mercer BM, D’alton M, Kilpatrick S, Blackwell S, Saade G. Timing of indicated late-preterm and early-term birth. Obstet Gynecol. 2011;118(2 pt 1): 323–333 5. Raju T, Higgins R, Stark A, Leveno K. Optimizing care and outcome for latepreterm (near-term) infants: a summary of the workshop sponsored by the National Institute of Child Health and Human Development. Pediatrics. 2006; 118(3):1207–1214 6. Teune MJ, Bakhuizen S, Gyamfi Bannerman C, et al. A systematic review of severe morbidity in infants born late preterm. Am J Obstet Gynecol. 2011;205 (4):374.e1–e9 7. Brown HK, Speechley KN, Macnab J, Natale R, Campbell MK. Neonatal morbidity associated with late preterm and early term birth: the roles of gestational age and biological determinants of preterm birth. Int J Epidemiol. 2014;43(3):802–814 8. Kerstjens JM, de Winter AF, BoccaTjeertes IF, ten Vergert EMJ, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children at school entry. J Pediatr. 2011;159(1):92–98 9. Morse SB, Zheng H, Tang Y, Roth J. Early school-age outcomes of late preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2009;123(4). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/ 123/4/e622

6

10. Petrini JR, Dias T, McCormick MC, Massolo ML, Green NS, Escobar GJ. Increased risk of adverse neurological development for late preterm infants. J Pediatr. 2009;154(2):169–176 11. McGowan JE, Alderdice FA, Holmes VA, Johnston L. Early childhood development of late-preterm infants: a systematic review. Pediatrics. 2011;127(6): 1111–1124 12. Schonhaut L, Perez M, Schonstedt M, et al. Moderately and late preterm newborns, a risk group for low cognitive development in the first years of life. Rev Chil Pediatr. 2012;83(4):358–365 13. Poulsen G, Wolke D, Kurinczuk JJ, et al. Gestational age and cognitive ability in early childhood: a population-based cohort study. Paediatr Perinat Epidemiol. 2013;27(4):371–379 14. Kirkegaard I, Obel C, Hedegaard M, Henriksen TB. Gestational age and birth weight in relation to school performance of 10-year-old children: a follow-up study of children born after 32 completed weeks. Pediatrics. 2006;118(4):1600–1606 15. Talge NM, Holzman C, Wang J, Lucia V, Gardiner J, Breslau N. Late-preterm birth and its association with cognitive and socioemotional outcomes at 6 years of age. Pediatrics. 2010;126(6):1124–1131 16. Noble KG, Fifer WP, Rauh VA, Nomura Y, Andrews HF. Academic achievement varies with gestational age among children born at term. Pediatrics. 2012; 130(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/ cgi/content/full/130/2/e257 17. Dong Y, Chen SJ, Yu JL. A systematic review and meta-analysis of long-term development of early term infants. Neonatology. 2012;102(3):212–221 18. Rose O, Blanco E, Martinez SM, et al. Developmental scores at 1 year with increasing gestational age, 37-41 weeks. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/131/ 5/e1475 19. Woythaler MA, McCormick MC, Smith VC. Late preterm infants have worse 24-month neurodevelopmental outcomes than term infants. Pediatrics. 2011;

127(3). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/ cgi/content/full/127/3/e622 20. Romeo DM, Di Stefano A, Conversano M, et al. Neurodevelopmental outcome at 12 and 18 months in late preterm infants. Eur J Paediatr Neurol. 2010;14(6): 503–507 21. Walsh JM, Doyle LW, Anderson PJ, Lee KJ, Cheong JLY. Moderate and late preterm birth: effect on brain size and maturation at term-equivalent age. Radiology. 2014;273(1):232–240 22. Kerstjens JM, Bocca-Tjeertes IF, de Winter AF, Reijneveld SA, Bos AF. Neonatal morbidities and developmental delay in moderately preterm-born children. Pediatrics. 2012;130(2). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/130/ 2/e265 23. Gurka MJ, LoCasale-Crouch J, Blackman JA. Long-term cognition, achievement, socioemotional, and behavioral development of healthy late-preterm infants. Arch Pediatr Adolesc Med. 2010; 164(6):525–532 24. Baron IS, Erickson K, Ahronovich MD, Baker R, Litman FR. Cognitive deficit in preschoolers born late-preterm. Early Hum Dev. 2011;87(2):115–119 25. McGowan JE, Alderdice FA, Doran J, et al. Impact of neonatal intensive care on late preterm infants: developmental outcomes at 3 years. Pediatrics. 2012; 130(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/ cgi/content/full/130/5/e1105 26. Vohr B. Long-term outcomes of moderately preterm, late preterm, and early term infants. Clin Perinatol. 2013; 40(4):739–751 27. Squires J, Bricker D. Ages and Stages Questionnaires, Third Edition (ASQ-3): A Parent-Completed Child-Monitoring System. Baltimore, MD: Brookes Publishing Company; 2009 28. Ministerio de Salud. Normas técnicas de evaluación y estimulación del desarrollo psicomotor en el niño y la niña menor de 6 años [Technical standards of evaluation and stimulation of psychomotor development in boys and girls under 6 years of age]. Santiago

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

SCHONHAUT et al

Chile 2004. Available at: http://web. minsal.cl/sites/default/files/files/2004_ Normas_tecnicas_estimulacion_y_ evaluacion_desarrollo.pdf. Accessed February 9, 2015 29. Richter J, Janson H. A validation study of the Norwegian version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires. Acta Paediatr. 2007;96(5):748–752 30. Juneja M, Mohanty M, Jain R, Ramji S. Ages and Stages Questionnaire as a screening tool for developmental delay in Indian children. Indian Pediatr. 2012; 49(6):457–461 31. Filgueiras A, Pires P, Maissonette S, Landeira-Fernandez J. Psychometric properties of the Brazilian-adapted version of the Ages and Stages Questionnaire in public child daycare centers. Early Hum Dev. 2013;89(8): 561–576 32. Schonhaut L, Armijo I, Schönstedt M, Alvarez J, Cordero M. Validity of the Ages and Stages Questionnaires in term and preterm infants. Pediatrics. 2013;131(5). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/ content/full/131/5/e1468 33. Adimark. Mapa Socioecónico de Chile. 2004. Available at: www.adimark.cl/ medios/estudios/mapa_ socioeconomico_de_chile.pdf. Accessed December 4, 2014

births with child health and development: Japanese populationbased study. J Pediatr. 2013;163(6): 1578–1584.e4 37. Soria-Pastor S, Padilla N, ZubiaurreElorza L, et al. Decreased regional brain volume and cognitive impairment in preterm children at low risk. Pediatrics. 2009;124(6). Available at: www.pediatrics. org/cgi/content/full/124/6/e1161 38. Kerstjens JM, de Winter AF, BoccaTjeertes IF, Bos AF, Reijneveld SA. Risk of developmental delay increases exponentially as gestational age of preterm infants decreases: a cohort study at age 4 years. Dev Med Child Neurol. 2012;54(12):1096–1101 39. Mathiasen R, Hansen B, Nybo Andersen AM, Forman J, Greisen G. Gestational age and basic school achievements: a national follow-up study in Denmark. Pediatrics. 2010;126(6). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/126/ 6/e1553 40. MacKay DF, Smith GCS, Dobbie R, Pell JP. Gestational age at delivery and special educational need: retrospective cohort study of 407,503 schoolchildren. PLoS Med. 2010;7(6):e1000289

34. Milad M, Novoa JM, Fabres J, Samamé M, Aspillaga C. Recomendación sobre Curvas de Crecimiento Intrauterino. Rev Chil Pediatr. 2010;81(3):264–274

41. Shapiro-Mendoza C, Kotelchuck M, Barfield W, et al. Enrollment in early intervention programs among infants born late preterm, early term, and term. Pediatrics. 2013;132(1). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/cgi/content/full/132/ 1/e61

35. Marks K, Hix-Small H, Clark K, Newman J. Lowering developmental screening thresholds and raising quality improvement for preterm children. Pediatrics. 2009;123(6):1516–1523

42. Bhutta AT, Cleves MA, Casey PH, Cradock MM, Anand KJS. Cognitive and behavioral outcomes of school-aged children who were born preterm: a meta-analysis. JAMA. 2002;288(6):728–737

36. Kato T, Yorifuji T, Inoue S, Yamakawa M, Doi H, Kawachi I. Associations of preterm

43. Kinney HC. The near-term (late preterm) human brain and risk for periventricular

leukomalacia: a review. Semin Perinatol. 2006;30(2):81–88 44. Kapellou O, Counsell SJ, Kennea N, et al. Abnormal cortical development after premature birth shown by altered allometric scaling of brain growth. PLoS Med. 2006;3(8):e265 45. Kugelman A, Colin AA. Late preterm infants: near term but still in a critical developmental time period. Pediatrics. 2013;132(4):741–751 46. Guevara JP, Gerdes M, Localio R, et al. Effectiveness of developmental screening in an urban setting. Pediatrics. 2013; 131(1):30–37 47. Hintz SR, Kendrick DE, Vohr BR, Kenneth Poole W, Higgins RD; NICHD Neonatal Research Network. Gender differences in neurodevelopmental outcomes among extremely preterm, extremely-lowbirthweight infants. Acta Paediatr. 2006; 95(10):1239–1248 48. Ryan-Krause P, Meadows-Oliver M, Sadler L, Swartz MK. Developmental status of children of teen mothers: contrasting objective assessments with maternal reports. J Pediatr Health Care. 2009; 23(5):303–309 49. Janus M, Goldberg S. Factors influencing family participation in a longitudinal study: comparison of pediatric and healthy samples. J Pediatr Psychol. 1997; 22(2):245–262 50. Lindström K, Lindblad F, Hjern A. Psychiatric morbidity in adolescents and young adults born preterm: a Swedish national cohort study. Pediatrics. 2009; 123(1). Available at: www.pediatrics.org/ cgi/content/full/123/1/e47 51. Moster D, Lie RT, Markestad T. Long-term medical and social consequences of preterm birth. N Engl J Med. 2008;359(3): 262–273

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

PEDIATRICS Volume 135, number 4, April 2015

7

Gestational Age and Developmental Risk in Moderately and Late Preterm and Early Term Infants Luisa Schonhaut, Iván Armijo and Marcela Pérez Pediatrics; originally published online March 2, 2015; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1957 Updated Information & Services

including high resolution figures, can be found at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2015/02/24 /peds.2014-1957

Subspecialty Collections

This article, along with others on similar topics, appears in the following collection(s): Focus on Subspecialties http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/cgi/collection/focus

Permissions & Licensing

Information about reproducing this article in parts (figures, tables) or in its entirety can be found online at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/Permissions.xh tml

Reprints

Information about ordering reprints can be found online: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/site/misc/reprints.xhtml

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

Gestational Age and Developmental Risk in Moderately and Late Preterm and Early Term Infants Luisa Schonhaut, Iván Armijo and Marcela Pérez Pediatrics; originally published online March 2, 2015; DOI: 10.1542/peds.2014-1957

The online version of this article, along with updated information and services, is located on the World Wide Web at: http://pediatrics.aappublications.org/content/early/2015/02/24/peds.2014-1957

PEDIATRICS is the official journal of the American Academy of Pediatrics. A monthly publication, it has been published continuously since 1948. PEDIATRICS is owned, published, and trademarked by the American Academy of Pediatrics, 141 Northwest Point Boulevard, Elk Grove Village, Illinois, 60007. Copyright © 2015 by the American Academy of Pediatrics. All rights reserved. Print ISSN: 0031-4005. Online ISSN: 1098-4275.

Downloaded from pediatrics.aappublications.org at UCSF Library & CKM on March 10, 2015

Gestational age and developmental risk in moderately and late preterm and early term infants.

The objective of this study was to evaluate the association between gestational age (GA) at birth and risk of developmental delay at 8 and 18 months o...
735KB Sizes 0 Downloads 9 Views