RESEARCH ARTICLE

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape: Exploring the Effects of Taskscape Visibility on Cultural Diversity L. S. Premo1,2*, Gilbert B. Tostevin3

a11111

1 Department of Anthropology, Washington State University, Pullman, Washington, United States of America, 2 Department of Human Evolution, Max Planck Institute for Evolutionary Anthropology, Leipzig, Germany, 3 Department of Anthropology, University of Minnesota, Minneapolis, Minnesota, United States of America * [email protected]

Abstract OPEN ACCESS Citation: Premo LS, Tostevin GB (2016) Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape: Exploring the Effects of Taskscape Visibility on Cultural Diversity. PLoS ONE 11(9): e0161766. doi:10.1371/journal. pone.0161766 Editor: Michael D. Petraglia, Max Planck Institute for the Science of Human History, GERMANY Received: February 17, 2016 Accepted: August 11, 2016 Published: September 1, 2016 Copyright: © 2016 Premo, Tostevin. This is an open access article distributed under the terms of the Creative Commons Attribution License, which permits unrestricted use, distribution, and reproduction in any medium, provided the original author and source are credited. Data Availability Statement: All relevant data are within the paper and its Supporting Information files. S1 File provides the source code needed to replicate the findings presented in the study. The source code runs in NetLogo, an agent-based modeling platform freely available for download at https://ccl. northwestern.edu/netlogo/. S2 File provides all of the data presented in Figs 2–6. Funding: LSP was supported by Washington State University. GBT was supported by University of Minnesota. The funders had no role in study design, data collection and analysis, decision to publish, or preparation of the manuscript.

Culturally transmitted behavior can be structured in its performance both geographically and temporally, in terms of where and when implements are made and used on the landscape (what Ingold calls “the taskscape”). Yet cultural transmission theory has not yet explored the consequences of behaviors transmitted differently due to their enactment at different taskscape locations, what Tostevin calls “taskscape visibility.” Here, we use computer simulations to explore how taskscape visibility and forager mobility affect the diversity of two selectively neutral culturally transmitted traits within a single population of social learners. The trait that can be transmitted from residential bases only (lower taskscape visibility) shows greater diversity than the trait that can be transmitted from residential bases and logistical camps (higher taskscape visibility). In addition, increased logistical mobility has a positive effect on the diversity of the trait with the lower taskscape visibility while it generally shows little to no effect on the diversity of the trait with higher taskscape visibility. Without an appreciation for the ways in which taskscape visibility and mobility can structure cultural transmission in space and through time, the difference in the observed equilibrium diversity levels of the two traits might be incorrectly interpreted as resulting from qualitatively different forms of biased cultural transmission. The results of our simulation experiment suggest that researchers may need to take the taskscape visibility into account when inferring cultural transmission from archaeological data.

Introduction The dialogue between cultural transmission (CT) theorists and archaeologists over the last thirty years has been immensely stimulating. Researchers into CT theory have made significant progress investigating the effects of different rules for choosing role models from whom to learn [1–3]. They have done an excellent job explaining why cultural transmission is markedly different from a gene-like memetic view of transmission (compare [4,5] with [6,7]). They have even begun to advance models for the prehistoric evolution of shared norms and ethnic

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

1 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Competing Interests: The authors have declared that no competing interests exist.

differentiation [8,9], the appearance and maintenance of social stratification [10], and the effect of demography on cultural evolution [11–13]. And yet there are still many aspects of cultural transmission worthy of further examination. To the best of our knowledge CT theory has not yet formally explored the consequences of observations by ethnoarchaeologists and behavioral archaeologists that certain behaviors or tasks tend to be performed only at specific times and places, and thus for a specific “audience” of potential learners [14,15]. The fact that hunter-gatherers execute different tasks in different locations at different times has been a core principle for interpreting the archaeological record since the second half of the twentieth century. Long before Binford débuted processual archaeology’s engagement with landscape and the behavioral component of site formation processes [16,17], Willey’s [18] pioneering example of settlement pattern analysis in the Virú Valley of Peru was inspiring archaeologists to investigate cultural systems through their behavioral manifestation across landscapes. Subsequent ethnoarchaeological research developed new ways to understand how humans plan their utilization of geographically situated resources (including other people) throughout the year [19–26]. From a different perspective, archaeologists such as Butzer [27] and Schiffer [28] focused attention on how those plans interact structurally with the geological processes of a given location to preserve (or not) a behavior in the archaeological record. Between these perspectives, archaeologists have long been concerned with why we see a particular behavior in certain contexts but not others. It is our observation that much of the geographical specificity of learned behavior remains underutilized by the CT research applied to the archaeological record, with the exception of a few relatively recent examples [29–32].

Cultural transmission on the taskscape Many archaeologists involved in CT research draw from a branch of processual archaeology one can roughly equate to the Organization of Technology, with deep roots in Binford [16,20,33], Bleed [34], Shott [35,36], Nelson [37], and Kelly [38]. This approach continues to be immensely useful for addressing many interesting research questions. But it tends to focus on the fitness effects of technology across landscapes rather than on the social component of technology, or more specifically on how behavior impacts the transmission of a technology (for a longer discussion, see [15]:49–61). In this paper we utilize the social anthropological term taskscape rather than the more familiar term landscape. Social anthropologist Tim Ingold [39] advanced taskscape in an effort to argue for unison of interest between social anthropology and archaeology through the subject of the temporality of landscapes. In so doing, he emphasized the social component of spatially situated behavior as central to his purpose: . . .one of the outstanding features of human technical practices lies in their embeddedness in the current of sociality. It is to the entire ensemble of tasks, in their mutual interlocking, that I refer by the concept of taskscape. Just as the landscape is an array of related features, so—by analogy—the taskscape is an array of related activities. ([39]:158, italics in original) This emphasis on interlocking tasks would not be amiss in ethnoarchaeological accounts of the Nunamiut [19] or! Kung San [26]. And yet the taskscape concept alone does not allow us to predict how the specific content and context of a task should affect its transmission. For this we need to look to Carr’s “Unified Middle-Range Theory of Artifact Design” [40]. Using a combination of ethnoarchaeological observations (e.g., [25]), Wobst’s [41] information exchange approach to style theory, and a practical orientation to the attribute analysis of artifacts, Carr argued that one can follow an

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

2 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

analytical process to identify which social processes could have affected the variability of a given attribute of an artifact based on its material constraints. Beyond the most basic of functional requirements, the attribute is evaluated along each of three hierarchies that define its material constraints: i) its place in the order of decisions made by the maker (e.g., an earlier decision can limit later decisions in the decision hierarchy); ii) its place in the production sequence for the artifact (e.g., earlier behaviors are frequently obscured in the final product by later steps); and iii) its physical visibility to observers (e.g., small attributes cannot be seen from long distances). The hierarchies are evaluated sequentially into a final prediction for how the attribute could vary under different social processes. Given Ingold’s focus on how technical performances are distributed on the taskscape and Carr’s emphasis on the necessity to witness such acts in order to learn them, Tostevin advanced the combined term taskscape visibility to emphasize that details concerning where, when, and ultimately for whom a cultural trait is performed can affect its transmission (see [14]:345 and [15]:85). In order to systematically explore how taskscape visibility can structure cultural transmission, here we study the transmission of two selectively neutral cultural traits (A and B) that differ only in taskscape visibility. Trait A is visible only at residential bases (such as the core reduction details for debitage blanks for the making of curated retouched tools) while trait B is visible both at residential bases and logistical camps (such as the morphological shape of curated retouched tools made at residential bases and transported to logistical camps for use). The results of our simulation experiment suggest that taskscape visibility may be considered another potential form of “bias” unique to cultural transmission.

On the relationship between cultural transmission, effective population size, and cultural diversity In the absence of natural or cultural selection, the equilibrium diversity of a discrete cultural trait in a finite population is a function of the balance between the rate at which variation is lost due to drift and the rate at which variation is introduced via copying errors (and/or transmission from a different population). Motoo Kimura and James Crow [42,43] formalized the relationship between drift, copying error, and the equilibrium diversity of a discrete trait. As recounted by Neiman [44], the equilibrium diversity (θ) of a discrete cultural trait is given by: y ¼ 2Ne m;

ð1Þ

where Ne is the effective population size of the trait of interest and μ is the probability per transmission event that a novel variant of the trait is introduced to the population. Eq 1 makes clear that the equilibrium diversity of a cultural trait is a function of its effective population size rather than its census population size. Effective population size is a standardized measure that represents the size of the idealized Wright-Fisher population that would show the same magnitude of drift as occurs in the trait of interest in the real population [45]. Traits marked by larger Ne lose variation at a slower rate than those characterized by smaller Ne. Assuming equivalent μ, traits with a larger Ne maintain a greater level of equilibrium diversity than traits with smaller Ne. Crow and Kimura [46] show that inbreeding effective population size can be calculated from demographic data as follows: Nk  1 ; N e ¼ Vk þk1 k

ð2Þ

where N is the size of the parent generation, k is mean number of progeny per member of the

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

3 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

parent generation, and Vk is variance in number of progeny per member of the parent generation. Eq 2 can be used to calculate the effective population size of a cultural trait if one extends “parent” to teacher and “progeny” to learner. Eq 2 reveals that whether the effective population size of a trait is less than, greater than, or equal to N in a constant, finite population depends upon Vk (see [46]:110–111). Under the idealized conditions of the standard neutral WrightFisher model the sampling variation in k is given by a binomial probability distribution, Vk = k(1 − 1/N), and Ne = N. However, the effective population size of a trait can depart from N under non-ideal conditions, including those studied in this paper. What we have presented in this section sets the stage for an intuitive but important point: traits passed via different mechanisms of cultural transmission within the same population of social learners can show different levels of diversity because they are marked by different effective population sizes. Some forms of biased cultural transmission push Vk above binomial variance (thereby decreasing Ne relative to N), while others reduce Vk below binomial variance (thereby increasing Ne relative to N). For example, holding all else constant, a trait passed via directly biased oblique cultural transmission is marked by a lower effective population size (Ne) and thus lower equilibrium diversity (θ) than a trait passed via unbiased transmission, even when both traits are transmitted within the same population. Of course, factors other than directly biased cultural transmission can push Vk above (e.g., anti-conformist frequency dependent transmission) or below (e.g., prestige based bias) binomial variance. Below we use a spatially explicit model to show that taskscape visibility can also affect the diversity of selectively neutral cultural traits in a structured population of central-place foragers.

The Model The terms landscape and taskscape emphasize different aspects of a fundamentally spatial concept. The concept of taskscape visibility draws attention to the fact that human behavior is partitioned in space, such that some activities take place only in certain parts of the taskscape. People who are not in close proximity to such places may not have access to the knowledge or technology used during the course of the activity. In short, one can be isolated by distance on a taskscape in much that same way that one can be isolated by distance on a landscape. We employ an agent-based model in this study because of the spatial nature of the concept of taskscape visibility. Agent-based models are especially well suited for representing interactions among heterogeneous actors in space. An added benefit is that agent-based models are relatively intuitive and easy to communicate to a wider anthropological audience. Our spatially explicit agent-based model is designed to address the following research questions. First, holding the mobility strategy constant, how does variability in taskscape visibility affect the diversities of traits A and B in central-place foragers? Second, how does a shift to a more logistically oriented mobility strategy affect the diversities of traits that differ only in their taskscape visibilities? Our Netlogo [47] source code and full model description are attached as S1 File. An abbreviated model description fills the remainder of this section. Consider a population of N agents dispersed randomly over a 250 cells x 250 cells lattice that is wrapped around a torus to avoid edge effects. Each agent represents a small self-sufficient group of central-place foragers. It is assumed that forager groups are the same size and consume resources at the same rate. Forager groups cannot suffer local extinction, reproduce, fission, or fuse together during the course of a simulation run. Each cell of the grid may contain a resource that provides enough food to support one group for one time step. Resource density is given by the parameter d. If d = 0.75, then 75% of the cells (chosen randomly) contain food resources. Agents deplete resources. When a group consumes food, the food does not reappear in its cell until 800 time steps have passed. Thus, the food resource in this model represents a

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

4 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Fig 1. A simple model of central-place foraging and cultural transmission of two traits that differ in taskscape visibility. Each foraging group conducts logistical forays within distance re of its residential base (a) until the foraging area is depleted of resources. Cultural transmission occurs between groups that find themselves within distance ri of one another during the normal course of foraging. These encounters can occur between two residential bases (b), between two logistical camps (c), or between a residential base and a logistical camp (d). Whether the teacher is observed at its residential base or logistical camp determines which trait(s) the learner is able to acquire via oblique cultural transmission. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g001

slowly regenerating source of calories. The rate of resource regeneration and N are held constant over all simulations. Foraging decisions, such as how long to remain at a residential base and how far to move to a new residential base, follow the logic of Kelly’s [38] central-place foraging model (Fig 1A; see also [31]). Agents conduct logistical forays from their residential bases to procure food. Each group is allowed to consume only those resources located within the effective foraging radius, re, of its current residential base. Each group has no information about the state of resources outside its current foraging area. All groups have the same effective foraging radius during each simulation run. Just as in Kelly’s mathematical model [38], lower values of re correspond to strategies that emphasize residential mobility and higher values of re correspond to strategies that emphasize logistical mobility. Logistical mobility refers to the movement of people between a residential base and a logistical camp [16]. Each time step, each group randomly chooses a cell to serve as

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

5 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

its logistical camp from among the set of cells that satisfy two criteria: i) the cell is located within a distance re of the agent’s residential base and ii) the cell currently contains food. In the event that none of the cells within the foraging area contains food, the group relocates its residential base. This represents residential mobility. To avoid overlap between the new foraging area and recently depleted areas, residential camps are moved a distance of 2re + 1 in a heading defined by adding a value chosen randomly from a uniform distribution bound by −45 and 45 to the group’s previous heading. In other words, residential mobility follows a correlated random walk with a non-variable step length of 2re + 1. To better isolate the effect of re on cultural diversity we assume that all of the other needs that might require mobility (e.g., travel for water or for raw materials such as stone or firewood, or to share information with other groups) are embedded within logistical and residential mobility. A group cannot move logistically and residentially during the course of a single time step. Each group moves its residential base or conducts a logistical foray during each time step; it cannot do both. In the event that a residential move places a group in a foraging area that is completely devoid of food, that group will make another residential move during the subsequent time step. As stated above, the agents in our model represent small groups of central-place foragers. Because we focus on how the mobility of groups affects cultural diversity at the level of the metapopulation, we do not represent individual foragers. Instead, we make the simplifying assumption that all of the members of a foraging group display the same variant of each selectively neutral cultural trait. Different variants of each trait are represented by integers. Traits can be transmitted via oblique cultural transmission between groups that find themselves located within each other’s interaction radius, ri, during the course of the time step (Fig 1). There are a number of ways in which groups might encounter each other while foraging for food. Perhaps the most obvious case is that in which the residential bases of two or more groups are separated by a distance less than or equal to ri (see Fig 1B). It is also possible for foragers of different groups to encounter one another when their logistical camps are located within ri of each other (Fig 1C). Finally, logistical forays can bring a group into contact with another group’s residential base, just as a residential move can situate the group’s new base within ri of another group’s logistical camp (Fig 1D). In cases where at least one other group is located within ri of ego’s residential base or within ri of ego’s logistical camp, ego chooses one of the encountered groups (at random, if more than one were encountered) to serve as its teacher during the present time step. Whether the teacher is encountered at its residential base or at its logistical camp becomes important under the additional assumption that cultural traits A and B differ in their taskscape visibility. We assume that trait A can be observed, and thus transmitted, at residential bases only (as in the cases of Fig 1B and 1D). By contrast, trait B is less restricted. Trait B can be observed at, and thus transmitted from, residential bases (Fig 1B and 1D) and logistical camps (Fig 1C and 1D). In cases where there are no groups located within ri of ego’s residential base or within ri ego’s logistical camp, ego acquires variants of trait A and B via uniparental vertical cultural transmission [2] from the previous generation, or “parental,” version of itself. Note that regardless of whether a trait is passed via vertical or oblique cultural transmission, μ represents the probability per trait per group of a copying error. We assume an infinite-variants model of copying error, whereby each error introduces a novel variant (i.e., a unique integer) of the trait. At the start of each simulation run, d x 250 x 250 cells are seeded with food resources. Next, each foraging group is placed on a randomly chosen cell and initialized with a unique cultural variant at trait A and trait B. The richness of cultural variants at each trait is equal to N at the start of each simulation. The following methods occur in the following order during each iteration, or time step, of the simulation. First, groups forage. This involves either a logistical foray

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

6 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

to a cell within the group’s foraging area or a residential move to a cell outside of its foraging area, but not both. Affected cells are updated to reflect the fact that foragers deplete resources. Second, each group acquires variants for traits A and B via vertical or oblique cultural transmission. In either case, with probability μ per trait per transmission event the learner adopts a novel variant rather than the value displayed by its teacher. Third, food resources that have been absent for 800 time steps regenerate. One can think of each time step of the simulation as the time required for every group in the population to undergo social learning once (and only once). After 10,000 times steps—sufficient time for diversity in both traits to reach non-stable equilibrium in the metapopulation—data are collected and the simulation ends. We collect several different types of data at the end of each simulation run (S2 File). To investigate the effect of re on cultural diversity, we calculate tF for each trait in the final metapopulation of 25 groups. tF is an estimate of the equilibrium diversity of a given trait in a population (see [44] and Eq 3 below). To better understand the effect of re on the diversity of traits A and B, we also keep track of the total number of oblique cultural transmission events for each trait for the duration of each simulation. In addition, we track the “history” of oblique transmission events of each group—in particular, the number of times that each of the other 24 groups served as a teacher for ego. We use these cultural transmission “histories” to calculate the mean number of teachers per group as well as the mean coefficient of variation (CV) of times taught by each of the other 24 groups during the simulation. Thirty unique simulations were executed for each possible combination of parameter values (Table 1), resulting in a total of 1620 runs.

Results We begin with the results for d = 0.75. We present the results for d = 1, albeit in less detail because they are qualitatively similar to those for d = 0.75, near the end of this section.

Interaction radius (ri) affects the mix of vertical and oblique cultural transmission In a metapopulation in which groups are completely isolated from one another, the proportion of oblique transmission events (i.e., transmission between groups) is 0, as all social learning occurs via uniparental vertical cultural transmission from the “parental” to the “offspring” generation within each group. Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman called this “extreme” uniparental vertical transmission because every member of the parent generation transmits its variant to a different member of the offspring generation (see [2]:84–85). The other end of the spectrum is characterized by a freely mixing population, which is described by the assumptions of a standard Wright-Fisher model of transmission. The proportion of oblique transmission events is 1 − (1/N) in a freely mixing population. For N = 25, 0 and 0.96 mark boundary conditions in Table 1. Parameter values used in the experimental design. Parameters

Values

Number of foraging groups, N

25

Resource regenRate

800

Resource density, d

0.75, 1

Interaction radius, ri

5, 10, 15

Effective foraging radius, re

5, 10, 15

Copying error rate, μ

0.0001, 0.001, 0.01

Seed

1–30

doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.t001

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

7 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Fig 2. Increasing interaction radius ri increases the proportion of times that trait A (blue) and trait B (red) are passed via oblique cultural transmission. Each boxplot represents data collected from 30 unique simulation runs. The length of the effective foraging radius increases from the left panel to the right panel. The dashed line provides the value associated with the boundary condition of extreme uniparental vertical cultural transmission. The other boundary condition, represented by a Wright-Fisher population, is associated with a value of 0.96. Note that d = 0.75 and that these results are exactly the same for all values of μ. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g002

proportion of oblique transmission events associated with total isolation between groups and no isolation between groups, respectively. Fig 2 plots the proportion of oblique (i.e., intergroup) cultural transmission events per simulation run against ri for each of the re values tested. The observed proportion of oblique (i.e., intergroup) transmission events is closer to the uniparental vertical transmission boundary than to the Wright-Fisher boundary for both traits in our model (Fig 2). Holding all else constant, increasing ri increases the proportion of oblique transmission events (and thus decreases the proportion of uniparental vertical transmission events) for both traits. Also note that trait B is passed via oblique cultural transmission more frequently than trait A under the conditions tested here. This result makes sense given that trait B can be passed from residential bases and logistical camps while trait A can be passed from residential bases only. Regardless of the value of re, increasing the interaction radius can only increase the likelihood of intergroup interaction in a spatially explicit setting [30,31]. Put differently, the conditions of our spatially explicit simulation approach the conditions of the Wright-Fisher population as ri approaches the spatial scale of the entire landscape. The ratio of vertical transmission events to oblique transmission events is important for understanding how ri and re affect diversity in selectively neutral traits. Recall that the equilibrium diversity (θ) of a discrete trait is a function of the effective population size (Ne) of that trait (Eq 1), which in turn is a function of Vk (Eq 2). Vk is binomial under the idealized conditions of unbiased cultural transmission (i.e., the freely mixing population). However, non-ideal conditions can cause Vk to depart from binomial variance. Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman (see [2]:192–202) have shown that under conditions in which at least some of the members of the offspring generation acquire their variant via uniparental vertical transmission rather than unbiased oblique transmission, Vk = α(2 − α), where α = zN/(N − 1). Here, z represents the probability that each member of the offspring generation acquires its variant via unbiased oblique cultural transmission and 1 − z the probability that each member of the offspring generation acquires its variant via uniparental vertical transmission. In the extreme case where all members of the offspring generation engage in uniparental vertical transmission rather than unbiased oblique transmission (z = 0), Vk = 0 and Ne is much greater than N. The general point here is that because the equilibrium diversity of a trait is sensitive to Ne and Ne is a function of Vk, the mix of oblique cultural transmission and uniparental vertical cultural transmission can affect the equilibrium diversity of a trait through its effect on Vk. More specifically, holding census population size constant, increasing the likelihood that members of the offspring

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

8 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

generation acquire their cultural traits via uniparental vertical transmission rather than via unbiased oblique cultural transmission increases the equilibrium diversity of the trait in question. The theory reviewed above allows one to formulate some general expectations concerning the diversity levels of traits A and B from the results presented in Fig 2. First, given that increasing the proportion of uniparental vertical cultural transmission relative to unbiased oblique transmission increases the effective population size and ultimately equilibrium diversity of a trait, trait A ought to show more diversity than trait B, holding all else constant. Second, because increasing ri increases the proportion of oblique cultural transmission events relative to uniparental vertical cultural transmission events, it follows that the equilibrium diversity of both traits should decrease as ri increases. It is more difficult to predict the effect of re on the diversity of traits A and B from Fig 2, but another recent study provides a reasonable place to start. For ri  2re, Premo [31] shows that increasing re reduces intergroup interaction and, thus, increases the effective population size (Ne) of a selectively neutral culturally transmitted trait. The positive relationship between Ne and θ (Eq 1) yields the prediction that increasing re increases the equilibrium diversity of traits A and B. It is unlikely to be quite that simple, however, because resource density (d) and the probability of making a copy error (μ) can regulate the effect of re on diversity. So, how do these predictions fare against the diversity data collected from spatially explicit populations on the taskscape? And, if there are departures from our expectations, what general lessons do they teach us about the significance of taskscape visibility in the context of cultural transmission?

Effective foraging radius (re) and taskscape visibility affect cultural diversity There are a number of ways to measure the diversity of a culturally transmitted trait. Richness—the number of variants of a discrete trait—is a simple but incomplete measure of diversity because it does not take into account the evenness with which the variants are represented. Neiman [44] introduces a useful method for estimating the equilibrium diversity, θ, of a trait from an empirical sample drawn from a standing population. His empirical estimate of θ, called tF, incorporates information on the richness and evenness of the variants displayed at a discretely varying trait: tF ¼

1 k X

 1; p

ð3Þ

2 i

i¼1

where pi is the relative frequency of the ith (i = 1, 2, 3,. . .k) variant of the trait observed in a sample (note that here k refers to the richness of cultural variants observed in the sample rather than to the number of progeny per member of the parent generation, as above). We calculate tF separately for traits A and B at the end of the 10,000th time step of each simulation run. Because our sample is equivalent to the entire population (in both the statistical and common senses of the word), we employ tF as an unbiased estimator of θ in this study. If every group displays the same variant of trait A, then tF = 0 for trait A. By contrast, if every group displays a unique variant of trait A, as would be the case when groups are completely isolated from one another and all transmission is vertical, then tF would take the highest possible value: N − 1. For reference we present the expected levels of diversity at the boundary conditions of total isolation among groups (tF = N − 1 = 24) and random copying (tF = 2Neμ, where Ne = N = 25) along with the cultural diversity observed at each trait. Fig 3 presents tF for traits A and B for d = 0.75. As predicted above, trait A shows greater diversity than trait B in all cases. Also as predicted, the diversity of both traits decreases as ri

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

9 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Fig 3. The effect of re on the diversity of trait A (blue) and trait B (red) for d = 0.75. Each boxplot represents 30 unique simulation runs. Interaction radius increases from the bottom row to the top row. Copy error rate increases from the left column to the right column. The dotted line in each panel provides the expected value of tF in a standard Wright-Fisher population. The dashed line in each panel provides the expected value of tF under extreme uniparental vertical cultural transmission. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g003

increases. The third prediction concerning the effect of effective foraging radius on diversity is generally met for the case of trait A. In nearly every case, increasing re has a significant positive effect on tF of trait A; the one exception is associated with p = 0.057 (Table 2). In other words, increasing re generally increases the diversity of trait A, pushing it closer to the value expected of a population in which traits are passed only by uniparental vertical cultural transmission.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

10 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Table 2. Results of regressing tF on re for d = 0.75. ri

μ

Trait A

Trait B

β

r2

p

β

r2

p

5

.0001

1.026

.761

< .001

-.035

.012

.314

5

.001

.882

.726

< .001

.017

.001

.807

5

.01

.201

.203

< .001

-.234

.104

.002

10

.0001

.426

.518

< .001

-.011

.002

.667

10

.001

.753

.579

< .001

-.019

.001

.764

10

.01

.269

.151

< .001

-.108

.023

.154

15

.0001

.188

.263

< .001

-.018

.008

.389

15

.001

.468

.394

< .001

-.015

.001

.757

15

.01

.156

.041

.057

-.243

.127

< .001

n = 90 runs in each regression. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.t002

However, the prediction that increasing re increases diversity is generally not met for trait B. To the contrary, our model results show that effective foraging radius has no effect on the diversity of trait B in the majority of cases (Table 2). There are two cases in which increasing re has a relatively weak but statistically significant negative effect on the diversity of trait B. For now, we merely note that both of these cases occur under the highest rate of copying error tested. That trait A displays more diversity than trait B is at least partially explained by the fact that trait A is transmitted obliquely (i.e., between groups) less frequently per unit time than is trait B (Fig 2). But because that difference holds true for all values of re, the fact that trait A is transmitted obliquely less frequently than trait B does not explain why increasing re affects the diversity of traits A and B differently. Thus, to understand why re often has a positive effect on the diversity of trait A but no effect on B requires one to investigate the role that taskscape visibility plays in structuring the transmission of traits A and B beyond just the number of times that each is transmitted obliquely versus vertically.

Taskscape visibility structures cultural transmission As shown above, Crow and Kimura [46] illustrate how variance in the number of progeny per member of the parent generation (Vk) affects effective population size (Ne) (Eq 2), which in turn affects equilibrium diversity (θ). This suggests that in order to investigate how mobility and taskscape visibility affect cultural diversity in our model it may be useful to consult data on the frequency with which each group transmits variants of traits A and B to learners. Or if one takes the perspective of the learner, as we do here, then one is interested in variance in the frequency with which each group learns from each of the other groups in the metapopulation over the course of a simulation. In the idealized case of a Wright-Fisher population each group can expect to learn from each of the 25 groups, including itself, an equal number of times (on average) through simulated time. In our case, this would mean that each group learns its variants of traits A and B from each of the 25 groups a total of 400 times over the course of 10,000 time steps. The distribution of the number of times taught by each of the other 24 groups can be characterized quickly and easily with the coefficient of variation (CV), calculated as the standard deviation of times taught by each of the other 24 groups divided by the mean times taught by each of the other 24 groups. In a Wright-Fisher population, the expected value of the CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups over 10,000 times steps is 0/400 = 0 for each group. The expected mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups averaged over all 25 groups is 0/25 = 0.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

11 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

The other boundary condition of interest is that in which groups are completely isolated from one another. In such an extremely “viscous” population, variants are acquired through uniparental vertical cultural transmission only; there is no oblique cultural transmission. Under such conditions, after 10,000 time steps each group will have acquired its variant through vertical transmission 10,000 times and from each of the other 24 groups 0 times. Unlike in the case of the Wright-Fisher population, here the CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups is undefined (0/0). In such a rigidly structured metapopulation of 25 groups, the effective population size of each culturally transmitted trait is infinitely large (i.e., drift is absent) and tF of each trait takes the maximum value of N − 1 = 24. Fig 4 plots the mean number of teachers (i.e., other groups from which ego learned at least once during the course of the simulation) per group against effective foraging radius. There are three things to note about these results. First, holding ri constant, increasing re decreases the mean number of teachers per group. In other words, with higher re, each group encounters and learns from a smaller subset of the other 24 groups on the taskscape (on average) over the course of 10,000 time steps. This is consistent with the results of earlier studies with similar models [30,31], which show that increasing logistical mobility at the expense of residential mobility reduces the scope of interaction between groups of central-place foragers. Second, controlling for ri and re, the mean number of teachers for trait A is less than the mean number of teachers for trait B. Again, this is as expected, given that trait B can be transmitted from residential bases and logistical camps while trait A can be transmitted from residential bases only. Third, and most importantly for our purposes here, the magnitude of the negative effect of re on mean number of teachers is greater for trait A than for trait B (Table 3). In other words, while increasing re decreases mean number of teachers for both traits, the magnitude of the negative effect is greater for trait A than B for all ri values tested. In a sense, increasing re structures the transmission of trait A (lower taskscape visibility) to a greater extent than it structures the transmission of trait B (higher taskscape visibility). Fig 4 shows that as re increases, each group (on average) learns from a smaller subset of the other 24 groups due to increased isolation by geographic distance. This is important, but the effect of re on the distribution of times taught by each of the other 24 groups is perhaps even

Fig 4. Increasing re decreases the mean number of teachers per group for both trait A (blue) and trait B (red). Each boxplot represents 30 unique simulation runs. The length of the interaction radius increases from the left panel to the right panel. The dotted line provides the expected value in an idealized Wright-Fisher population. The dashed line provides the expected value under extreme uniparental vertical cultural transmission. Note that d = 0.75 and that these results are exactly the same for all values of μ. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g004

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

12 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Table 3. Results of regressing mean number of teachers per group on re for d = 0.75. Trait A

ri

Trait B

β

r2

p

β

r2

p

5

-.954

.919

< .001

-.292

.685

< .001

10

-1.185

.970

< .001

-.363

.807

< .001

15

-.926

.963

< .001

-.348

.872

< .001

n = 90 runs in each regression. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.t003

more informative. Fig 5 plots the mean CV of times taught by each of the 24 other groups against re. Higher mean CV values are indicative of greater population structure, which in turn increases the effective population size and ultimately the equilibrium diversity of each of the cultural traits. There are four things to note about these results. First, holding ri constant, increasing re increases the mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups for both traits. Second, controlling for ri and re, the mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups is greater for trait A than for trait B. Third, the magnitude of the positive effect of re on mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups is greater for trait A than trait B for all ri values tested. Finally, increasing ri reduces the magnitude of the effect of re on mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups for both traits, though to a greater extent for A than B (Table 4). The results in Figs 4 and 5 and Tables 3 and 4 show that the difference between the taskscape visibilities of trait A and trait B is responsible for how re affects the structure of cultural transmission for each trait. While increasing re decreases the mean number of teachers per group (Fig 4) and increases the mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups (Fig 5) for both traits, the magnitude of this effect is greater for trait A than it is for trait B. Thus, the trait that can be observed only at residential bases shows greater sensitivity to increased logistical mobility than the trait with a higher taskscape visibility. These results are consistent with those of a related study that show the magnitude of the negative effect of re on the probability

Fig 5. Increasing re increases the mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups for both trait A (blue) and trait B (red). Each boxplot represents 30 unique simulation runs. The length of the interaction radius increases from the left panel to the right panel. The dotted line provides the expected value in an idealized Wright-Fisher population (i.e., random copying). Note that d = 0.75 and that these results are exactly the same for all values of μ. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g005

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

13 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Table 4. Results of regressing mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups on re for d = 0.75. Trait A

ri

Trait B

β

r2

p

β

r2

p

5

.159

.881

< .001

.067

.833

< .001

10

.105

.931

< .001

.044

.869

< .001

15

.072

.939

< .001

.037

.856

< .001

n = 90 runs in each regression. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.t004

of intergroup interaction between central-place foragers is greater when interactions are restricted to residential bases than when interactions are allowed between any combination of residential bases and logistical camps (compare column A to column B of Figure 1 in [30]). Our results show that increasing logistical mobility has different effects on the equilibrium diversity of traits marked by different taskscape visibilities. But that is not all there is to the story because the magnitude of the effect of re on equilibrium diversity of any trait also depends in part upon the rate at which new variation is introduced to the population via copying error. High copying error rates can swamp the effects of drift and, thus, reduce or even negate the effect of re on diversity. To wit, the highest value of μ tested is associated with the weakest effects of re on the diversity of trait A (Table 2).

Resource density (d) and copying error (μ) modulate the effect of re on diversity We have focused on how the primary experimental parameter, re, affects the diversity of traits A and B when d = 0.75. Before moving on, it is informative to take a look at the effect of re on tF in the context of higher resource density, d = 1, and to address how μ regulates the effect of re on tF. Fig 6 presents tF for traits A and B under d = 1. The results are qualitatively similar to those collected from d = 0.75. In all cases, re has a statistically significant positive effect on the diversity of trait A while re has a non-significant effect on the diversity of trait B in all but three cases (Table 5). The magnitude of the effect in the cases of all three exceptions, two of which show a positive relationship and one a negative relationship, is relatively weak. Controlling for ri, re, and μ, tF values for both traits tend to be higher when d is larger. This is because the number of residential moves per group per time step decreases as d increases (e.g., [31]), resulting in greater isolation by distance and thus a greater effective population size for any trait that can be passed between groups. Fig 6 further illustrates the interplay between the relative strengths of two competing evolutionary forces: copying error and drift. As described above, new variants of traits A and B are introduced by copying error during cultural transmission. Just as the evolutionary force of mutation increases genetic diversity in living populations, copying error increases the cultural diversity of traits A and B in our simulated populations. Our model also includes drift, a countervailing evolutionary force that removes variation from a population. Drift is strengthened by factors that increase the scope of cultural transmission. We can identify two such factors in our spatially explicit model of central-place foraging groups. Fig 4 shows that increasing ri or decreasing re increases the mean number of teachers per group. Fig 5 shows that increasing ri or decreasing re decreases mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups. Thus, increasing the interaction radius or decreasing the effective foraging radius strengthens the effects of drift relative to μ. Without a source of new variation the effect of re on tF after 10,000 times steps is unlikely to be detectable even though increasing re decreases mean number of teachers per group and

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

14 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Fig 6. The effect of re on the diversity of trait A (blue) and trait B (red) for d = 1. Each boxplot represents 30 unique simulation runs. Interaction radius increases from the bottom row to the top row. Copy error rate increases from the left column to the right column. The dotted line in each panel provides the expected value of tF in an idealized Wright-Fisher population. The dashed line in each panel provides the expected value of tF under extreme uniparental cultural transmission. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.g006

increases mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups. Thus, one should expect very low μ to dampen the effect of re on diversity, simply because there will be so little diversity to affect at equilibrium. At the other extreme marked by high copying error rate (μ = .01), the diversity-increasing effects are strong relative to the diversity-reducing effects of drift. As borne out by our simulations (Tables 2 and 5), even though increasing re decreases mean number of

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

15 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Table 5. Results of regressing tF on re for d = 1. ri

μ

Trait A

Trait B

β

r2

p

β

r2

p .463

5

.0001

1.159

.786

< .001

.028

.006

5

.001

.718

.568

< .001

-.059

.007

.428

5

.01

.127

.096

.003

-.335

.194

< .001

10

.0001

.564

.592

< .001

.100

.102

.002

10

.001

.839

.582

< .001

-.003

< .001

.967

10

.01

.244

.141

< .001

-.061

.008

.390

15

.0001

.317

.455

< .001

.105

.155

< .001

15

.001

.717

.563

< .001

.058

.016

.231

15

.01

.294

.160

< .001

-.106

.029

.110

n = 90 runs in each regression. doi:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766.t005

teachers per group and increases mean CV of times taught by each of the other 24 groups, the effect of re on tF is much weaker (if present at all) when μ is high simply because drift is weak relative to copying error. It is not a coincidence that when copying error is relatively high (μ = .01) tF values begin to approach the maximum value of tF, regardless of re (Figs 3 and 6). Thus, a relatively high copying error rate can overwhelm the effects of drift under the conditions of our model.

Discussion Our results show that increasing logistical mobility (i.e., increasing re) decreases the scope of interaction among central-place foraging groups in a spatially explicit population. As effective foraging radius increases, each group learns disproportionately more frequently from a smaller subset of possible teachers. This shift in the structure of intergroup cultural transmission has interesting implications for the equilibrium diversity levels of culturally transmitted traits that differ only in taskscape visibility. Because effective population size is a property of a trait rather than of a population per se, different traits within the same population can display different effective population sizes [45]. A number of factors contribute to the effective population size of a culturally transmitted trait. As one might suspect, the mode of cultural transmission can structure interactions between teachers and learners within a population. For example, Cavalli-Sforza and Feldman [2] have shown that traits passed via “extreme” uniparental vertical cultural transmission display a much lower Vk (and thus a larger effective population size) than traits passed via unbiased oblique transmission. The results of our two-trait model of cultural transmission show that traits A and B exhibit different levels of diversity because of the ways their differing taskscape visibilities structure interactions between teachers and learners in population of central-place foraging groups. We draw two general lessons from the results of our simulation experiment. First, it may not be appropriate to expect the equilibrium diversity levels of all culturally transmitted traits in a population to be predicted by a single effective population size. Such an expectation is valid only where the traits of interest are “linked”—that is, passed together via the same mechanism of cultural transmission. Because every culturally transmitted trait in a population has its own effective population size, defined in part by the way in which the trait was (or is) transmitted, we suggest that perhaps it is more pragmatic to assume that Ne and thus equilibrium diversity can (and probably will) vary among many of the cultural traits displayed within the same population. This observation holds important implications for

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

16 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

designing more powerful empirical tests of the so-called “demographic hypothesis” for cultural diversity (see [45] for a longer discussion). Second, to the extent that the transmission of a cultural trait was structured by the taskscape, taskscape visibility should be considered along with, not instead of, other factors when attempting to infer past cultural transmission from archaeological data concerning that trait. Ignoring taskscape visibility by assuming that any trait can be passed during any interaction regardless of where on the taskscape the interaction takes place could potentially lead one astray in inferring which mechanism of cultural transmission best explains the observed diversity. Consider how one might interpret the results of our own simulation experiment in the absence of information that trait B has a higher taskscape visibility than trait A. One might reasonably (but incorrectly) conclude trait A shows greater diversity because it was passed via a form of anti-conformist biased transmission while trait B shows less diversity because it was affected by prestige-based bias, for example. In this case, we know the difference between the observed diversities of traits A and B is explained solely by variation in taskscape visibility rather than cognition-related biases related to the relative frequency of cultural variants or the status of potential teachers. This is not to imply that taskscape visibility is the only, or even the most important, aspect of cultural transmission. Our study simply shows that taskscape visibility can affect the equilibrium diversity of traits that differ only in that respect. We submit that taskscape visibility is one of many important aspects of cultural transmission that archaeologists and CT researchers might need to consider when addressing cultural transmission in the archaeological record. Clearly the model conditions studied here are but a small subset of those that might have been representative of Pleistocene hominins. It was not our goal to build a “realistic” model of Pleistocene forager intergroup cultural transmission, a task we feel the temporal and spatial resolution of empirical data germane to the topic preclude. Rather we set out to build a heuristic model complete with an experimental design tailored to address our research question. That is to say, for us the question is the thing (apologies to Shakespeare). Although as with any model our results must be understood within the context of the assumptions that generated them, thinking critically about how our results might differ under slightly different assumptions is a highly constructive exercise that provides welcome perspective on the generality of our findings. The brief list of alternative assumptions considered below is meant to be illustrative and thought provoking rather than exhaustive. Our model is built around the pragmatic assumption that central-place foraging and taskscape visibility are the only factors determining the structure of a trait’s transmission. But obviously there can be more to cultural transmission than mobility and taskscape visibility, even when pertinent taskscape locations differ. For a case in point, Apel’s eloquent example of what is required to learn how to produce Late Neolithic flint daggers in Scandinavia shows how the different requirements for easily acquired declarative knowledge (connaissance) versus the more time-intensive acquisition of body technique know-how (savoir-faire) affect the “learnability,” and thus transmission, of early versus late stage reduction behaviors [48]. Apel argued that master knappers intentionally obfuscated and aggrandized the learning process in the eyes of the general public by hiding the more easily learned production behaviors (requiring mostly connaissance) in sites far from residential sites while pursuing their extremely difficult-to-learn finishing techniques (difficult because they required extensive savoir-faire) in the highly visible centers of residential sites. This example shows how the taskscape visibility concept can be far more complex than our relatively basic model allows, as Carr intended with his analogous concept of relative contextual visibility (see [40]:186). While not essential to our immediate research questions, this additional layer of complexity might be useful—even necessary—for studying cases such as Apel’s daggers.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

17 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

The simplifying assumption that all forager group movement in our simulation follows from Kelly’s central-place foraging model allows us to isolate how a shift in mobility strategy affects cultural diversity. To be clear, this assumption is pragmatic and does not reflect what we think actually happened in the past. Although we do not know what actually happened in the past, it seems uncontroversial to us that Pleistocene foragers likely undertook forays that were not directly related to procuring that day’s (or week’s) required calories. Reconnaissance trips with the goal of gathering information on the state of waterholes or other temporally sensitive resources might well have led foragers beyond their effective foraging radius. Excursions to distant quarries to procure the raw material used to make tools as well as trips to obtain goods, aid, or information from other groups also may have taken foragers outside their foraging areas (for an informative discussion of social mobility, see [49]). Of course, these kinds of forays could have facilitated intergroup interactions just as well as those with the goal of procuring calories. In addition, meeting with other groups to exchange mates or gifts as well as joining large seasonal aggregations for communal ceremonies at prearranged localities could have facilitated intergroup cultural transmission independently of central-place foraging. Although the extent to which this occurred during the Pleistocene remains largely unknown, we can say that our results concerning the effect of re on the cultural diversity of traits that differ only in their taskscape visibilities would be less applicable under conditions in which intergroup interaction—and, thus, intergroup cultural transmission—is largely decoupled from the mechanics of Kelly’s model of central-place foraging. Our model employs an infinite-variants model of copying error, whereby each instance of a copying error results in the introduction of a completely new variant of the cultural trait being transmitted. Because this assumption eliminates the possibility that a copying error introduces a variant that is currently (or was previously) displayed by any foraging group in the metapopulation, it allows for the highest possible level of diversity for a given set of model conditions. But there are other ways to represent copying error in discrete traits. According to the singlestepwise model each copying error increases or decreases the integer value of the target variant by 1. In a finite-variants model each copying error introduces a variant chosen randomly from a finite set of integers. Note that under these alternative models copying error can “introduce” a variant that is already present elsewhere in the metapopulation, yielding a lower level of diversity than the infinite-variants model, holding all else constant. As a result, the effect of re and taskscape visibility on cultural diversity might be weaker in the presence of either a singlestepwise or finite-variants model of copying error than what we observed under the infinitevariants model. Which model of copying error is most appropriate for a given trait is an empirical question that we think is best answered on a case-by-case (or more precisely, a trait-bytrait) basis. We further propose that it may be appropriate in some cases to apply qualitatively different models of copying error to different attributes of a single tool. Although our experimental design does not vary the number of foraging groups or the size of the “world” they inhabit, the effects of taskscape visibility and mobility described above are likely to be sensitive to the density of foraging groups on the landscape. Intergroup cultural transmission requires intergroup interaction, which at its most basic level is a function of the density, mobility, and interaction radius of foraging groups. Holding effective foraging radius and interaction radius constant, intergroup interaction rates will be higher when the density of groups on the landscape is higher. It stands to reason that the effects of taskscape visibility and mobility on cultural diversity should be weaker when the ratio of foraging groups to total cells is high and stronger when the ratio of foraging groups to total cells is low. The assertion that the effects of re and taskscape visibility on cultural diversity should weaken and ultimately disappear as the density of foraging groups in a region increases is consistent with our results. Although we did not vary N in our experiment, we did vary d, the density of food resources.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

18 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

Reducing d while holding N and the extent of the world constant effectively increases the density of groups in relation to the number of cells that contain food resources. As expected, the effect of re on cultural diversity is generally weaker when d = 0.75 than when d = 1. We concentrate on the results collected under d = 0.75 rather than d = 1 because the former is the more conservative value in the context of our experimental design, not because we think it is necessarily representative of the density of food resources during the Pleistocene. The estimation of the density of food resources from the empirical record of this time period is famously difficult, although notable efforts have been made [50].

Conclusion The effective population size of each culturally transmitted trait in a population is a function of the way in which interactions between teachers and learners are structured across space and through time. One can expect traits marked by different modes and mechanisms of cultural transmission to display different effective population sizes and, thus, different levels of equilibrium diversity even within the same population of social learners. This study has only explored selectively neutral traits and it must be acknowledged there are many content properties of cultural attributes that could structure their transmission beyond central-place foraging and taskscape visibility (such as the difficulty of mastery as savoir-faire vs. connaissance knowledge). Nevertheless, our results show that taskscape visibility can be an important factor in explaining equilibrium cultural diversity in a spatially explicit population of mobile social learners. This suggests that archaeological inferences regarding the mode and mechanism of cultural transmission may need to account for taskscape visibility, perhaps through a strategic choice of culturally transmitted attributes according to such content properties. Our findings place even more importance on improving our understanding of Paleolithic forager mobility, population density, and social network structure. We are among those who argue that our current understanding of such matters is, at best, largely incomplete and could certainly stand improvement. We also realize this is a tall order. But assuming for the moment that it is possible to gain a better purchase on such parameters empirically, additional rounds of spatially explicit models might allow us to home in on how such factors affect the diversity of culturally transmitted traits given assumptions regarding both how (i.e., biased transmission, unbiased transmission, etc.) and where (i.e., taskscape visibility) traits were passed on the taskscape.

Supporting Information S1 File. Source code. This.nlogo file runs in NetLogo versions 5.2 and 5.3. NetLogo is freely available from the World Wide Web. In addition to the fully commented source code, this file also contains a full description of the agent-based model as well as directions for replicating the results of our study. (ZIP) S2 File. Simulation data. This.txt file contains all of the data presented in Figs 2–6. (TXT)

Acknowledgments LSP would like to thank Prof. Dick Gomulkiewicz for many helpful discussions about population genetics. GBT would like to thank Prof. Bill Wimsatt for his inspiring discussions about cultural evolution, as well as the opportunity to bring LSP to the University of Minnesota to participate in GBT & Wimsatt’s joint course “Cultural Transmission & Archaeology” during

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

19 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

the latter’s tenure as Winton Chair in the Liberal Arts at the University of Minnesota. We are both grateful to two anonymous reviewers and our academic editor, Michael Petraglia, for their thoughtful critiques and suggestions.

Author Contributions Conceptualization: LSP GBT. Formal analysis: LSP. Investigation: LSP. Methodology: LSP GBT. Software: LSP. Validation: LSP. Writing – original draft: LSP GBT. Writing – review & editing: LSP GBT.

References 1.

Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Culture and the evolutionary process. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1985.

2.

Cavalli-Sforza L, Feldman M. Cultural transmission and evolution. Princeton: Princeton University Press; 1981.

3.

Richerson PJ, Boyd R. Not by genes alone. Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 2005.

4.

Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Meme theory oversimplifies cultural change. Scientific American 2000; 283: 54– 55.

5.

Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Memes: Universal acid or a better mousetrap? In: Aunger R, editor. Darwinizing culture: The status of memetics as a science. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 2000. pp. 143–162.

6.

Dawkins R. The selfish gene. Oxford: Oxford University Press; 1976.

7.

Blackmore S. The power of memes. Scientific American 2000; 283: 52–61.

8.

Henrich J, Boyd R. The evolution of conformist transmission and the emergence of between-group differences. Evol Hum Behav 1998; 19: 215–241.

9.

McElreath R., Boyd R, Richerson PJ. Shared norms and the evolution of ethnic markers. Curr Anthropol 2003; 44: 122–129.

10.

Henrich J, Boyd R. Division of labor, economic specialization, and the evolution of social stratification. Curr Anthropol 2008; 49: 715–724.

11.

Powell A, Shennan S, Thomas MG. Late Pleistocene demography and the appearance of modern human behavior. Science 2009; 324: 1298–1301. doi: 10.1126/science.1170165 PMID: 19498164

12.

Richerson PJ, Boyd R, Bettinger R. Cultural innovations and demographic change. Hum Biol 2009; 81: 211–235. doi: 10.3378/027.081.0306 PMID: 19943744

13.

Shennan S. Demography and cultural innovation: A model and its implications for the emergence of human culture. Camb Archaeol J 2001; 11: 5–16.

14.

Tostevin GB. Social intimacy, artefact visibility, and acculturation models of Neanderthal-modern human interaction. In: Mellars P, Boyle K, Bar-Yosef O, Stringer C, editors. Rethinking the human revolution: New behavioural and biological perspectives on the origins and dispersal of modern humans. Cambridge: MacDonald Institute Research Monograph; 2007. pp. 341–357.

15.

Tostevin GB. Seeing lithics: A middle-range theory for testing for cultural transmission in the Pleistocene. American School of Prehistoric Research Monograph Series, Peabody Museum, Harvard University. Oxford, UK, and Oakville, CT: Oxbow Books; 2012.

16.

Binford L. Willow smoke and dogs’ tails: Hunter-gatherer settlement systems and archaeological site formation. Am Antiq 1980; 45: 4–20.

17.

Binford L. The archaeology of place. J Anthropol Archaeol 1982; 1: 5–31.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

20 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

18.

Willey GR. Prehistoric settlement patterns in the Virú Valley, Peru. Bureau of American Ethnology Bulletin No. 155. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution; 1953.

19.

Binford L. Nunamiut ethnoarchaeology. New York: Academic Press; 1978.

20.

Binford L. Organization and formation processes: Looking at curated technologies. J Anthropol Res 1979; 35: 255–272.

21.

Gould RA. Living archaeology. New York: Cambridge University Press; 1980.

22.

Kent S. Studying variability in the archaeological record: An ethnoarchaeological model for distinguishing mobility patterns. Am Antiq 1992; 57: 635–660.

23.

O'Connell JF. Alyawara Site Structure and its Archaeological Implications. Am Antiq 1987; 52: 74–108.

24.

Wiessner P. Risk, reciprocity, and social influences on! Kung San economics. In: Leacock E, Lee RB, editors. Politics and history in band societies. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1982. pp. 61– 84.

25.

Wiessner P. Style and social information in Kalahari San projectile points. Am Antiq 1983; 48: 253–276.

26.

Yellen JE. Archaeological approaches to the present. New York: Academic Press; 1977.

27.

Butzer KW. Archaeology as human ecology. Cambridge: Cambridge University Press; 1982.

28.

Schiffer MB. Formation processes of the archaeological record. Albuquerque: University of New Mexico Press; 1987.

29.

Premo LS. Local extinctions, connectedness, and cultural evolution in structured populations. Adv Complex Syst 2012; 15:1150002:1–18.

30.

Premo LS. The shift to a predominantly logistical mobility strategy can inhibit rather than enhance forager interaction. Hum Ecol 2012; 40: 647–649.

31.

Premo LS. Mobility and cultural diversity in central-place foragers: Implications for the emergence of modern human behavior. In: Mesoudi A, Aoki K, editors. Learning strategies and cultural evolution during the Palaeolithic. Tokyo: Springer Press; 2015. pp. 45–65.

32.

Premo LS, Scholnick JB. The spatial scale of social learning affects cultural diversity. Am Antiq 2011; 75: 163–176.

33.

Binford L. Interassemblage variability—the Mousterian and the ‘functional’ argument. In: Renfrew C, editor. The explanation of culture change: Models in prehistory. London: Duckworth; 1973. pp. 227– 253.

34.

Bleed P. The optimal design of hunting weapons: Maintainability or reliability. Am Antiq 1986; 51: 547– 562.

35.

Shott M. Technological organization and settlement mobility: An ethnographic examination. J Anthropol Res 1986; 42: 15–51.

36.

Shott M. On tool-class use lives and the formation of archaeological assemblages. Am Antiq 1989; 54: 9–30.

37.

Nelson M. The Study of Technological Organization. Archaeol Method Theory 1991; 3: 57–100. Tucson: University of Arizona Press.

38.

Kelly RL. The foraging spectrum: Diversity in hunter-gatherer lifeways. Washington, D.C.: Smithsonian Institution Press; 1995.

39.

Ingold T. The temporality of the landscape. World Archaeol 1993; 25: 152–174.

40.

Carr C. A unified middle-range theory of artifact design. In: Carr C, Neitzel J, editors. Style, society, and person: Archaeological and ethnological perspectives. New York: Plenum Press; 1995. pp. 171–258.

41.

Wobst HM. Stylistic behavior and information exchange. In: Cleland C, editor. For the director: Essays in honor of James B. Griffin. Anthropological papers of the University of Michigan No. 61. Ann Arbor: Regents of the University of Michigan; 1977. pp. 317–342.

42.

Kimura M, Crow JF. The measurement of effective population number. Evolution 1963; 17: 279–288.

43.

Kimura M, Crow JF. The number of alleles that can be maintained in a finite population. Genetics 1964; 49: 725–738. PMID: 14156929

44.

Neiman F. Stylistic variation in evolutionary perspective: inferences from decorative diversity and interassemblage distance in Illinois woodland ceramic assemblages. Am Antiq 1995; 60: 7–36.

45.

Premo LS. Effective population size and the effects of demography on cultural diversity and technological complexity. Am Antiq 2016; In press.

46.

Crow JF, Kimura M. An introduction to population genetics theory. New York: Harper and Row; 1970.

47.

Wilensky U. NetLogo. Center for Connected Learning and Computer-Based Modeling, Evanston, IL: Northwestern University; 1999. http://ccl.northwestern.edu/netlogo/.

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

21 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape

48.

Apel J. Knowledge, know-how and raw material—The production of Late Neolithic flint daggers in Scandinavia. J Archaeol Method Theory 2008; 15: 91–111.

49.

Whallon R. Social networks and information: Non-“utilitarian” mobility among hunter-gatherers. J Anthropol Archaeol 2006; 25:259–270.

50.

Brown AG, Basell LS, Robinson S, Burdge GC. Site Distribution at the Edge of the Palaeolithic World: A Nutritional Niche Approach. PLoS ONE 2013; 8(12):e81476. doi: 10.1371/journal.pone.0081476 PMID: 24339935

PLOS ONE | DOI:10.1371/journal.pone.0161766 September 1, 2016

22 / 22

Cultural Transmission on the Taskscape: Exploring the Effects of Taskscape Visibility on Cultural Diversity.

Culturally transmitted behavior can be structured in its performance both geographically and temporally, in terms of where and when implements are mad...
2MB Sizes 0 Downloads 9 Views