Articles in PresS. J Neurophysiol (August 16, 2017). doi:10.1152/jn.00674.2016

1 2 3 4 5 6

Asymmetric vestibular stimulation reveals persistent disruption of motion

7

perception in unilateral vestibular lesions

8

R. Panichi1, M. Faralli, R2. Bruni1, A. Kiriakarely1, C. Occhigrossi1, A. Ferraresi1, A.M.

9

Bronstein3, V.E. Pettorossi1.

10 11 12

1Department

of Medicina Sperimentale, Sezione di Fisiologia Umana, Università di

13

Perugia, Perugia, Italy, 06129

14

2Department

15

Otorinolaringoiatria, Università di Perugia, Perugia, Italy, 06129

16

3Academic

17

Imperial College London, London, UK, W6 8RF

of Specialità Medico-Chirurgiche e Sanità Pubblica, Sezione di

Neuro-Otology, Centre for Neuroscience, Charing Cross Hospital,

18

Running title: motion perception and asymmetric vestibular stimulation

19 20 21

Corresponding author:

22

Prof. Adolfo M. Bronstein MD PhD FRCP FANA, Neuro-otology Unit, Division of

23

Brain Sciences, Imperial College London, Charing Cross Hospital.London W6 8RF,

24

[email protected]

25 26 27 28

0

Copyright © 2017 by the American Physiological Society.

29

Abstract

30

Self-motion perception was studied in patients with unilateral vestibular lesions

31

(UVL) due to acute vestibular neuritis at 1 week, 4, 8 and 12 months after the acute

32

episode. We assessed vestibularly-mediated self-motion perception by measuring

33

the error in reproducing the position of a remembered visual target at the end of 4

34

cycles of asymmetric whole-body rotation. The oscillatory stimulus consists of a

35

slow (0.09Hz) and a fast (0.38Hz) half cycle. A large error was present in UVL

36

patients when the slow half cycle was delivered towards the lesion side, but

37

minimal towards the healthy side. This asymmetry diminished over time, but it

38

remained abnormally large at 12 months. In contrast, vestibulo-ocular reflex

39

responses showed a large direction-dependent error only initially, then they

40

normalized. Normalization also occurred for conventional reflex vestibular

41

measures (caloric tests, subjective visual vertical and head shaking nystagmus) and

42

for perceptual function during symmetric rotation. Vestibular-related handicap,

43

measured with the Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI) at 12 months, correlated

44

with self-motion perception asymmetry but not with abnormalities in vestibulo-

45

ocular function. We conclude that 1) a persistent self-motion perceptual bias is

46

revealed by asymmetric rotation in UVLs despite vestibulo-ocular function

47

becoming symmetric over time 2) this dissociation is caused by differential

48

perceptual-reflex adaptation to high and low frequency rotations when these are

49

combined as with our asymmetric stimulus 3) the findings imply differential

50

central compensation for vestibulo-perceptual and vestibulo-ocular reflex

51

functions 4) self-motion perception disruption may mediate long-term vestibular-

52

related handicap in UVL patients.

53 54 55 56

New & Noteworthy

57

A novel vestibular stimulus, combining asymmetric slow and fast sinusoidal half

58

cycles, revealed persistent vestibulo-perceptual dysfunction in unilateral vestibular

59

lesion (UVL) patients. The compensation of motion perception after UVL was

1

60

slower than that of vestibulo ocular reflex. Perceptual but not vestibulo-ocular

61

reflex deficits correlated with dizziness-related handicap.

62 63

Keywords

64

Motion perception, asymmetric rotation, unilateral vestibular lesion, dizziness,

65

vestibular neuritis.

66

2

67

Introduction

68

Acute unilateral vestibular lesions (UVL), in man typically due to vestibular

69

neuritis, disrupt vestibular reflexes (including loss of gaze and postural stability),

70

as well as self-motion perception, represented initially by an intense rotational

71

sensation (vertigo) (Baloh 2003; Halmagy et al, 2010). After 48-72 hours symptoms

72

subside and vestibular reflexes gradually recover through a process of “central

73

vestibular compensation” which includes plastic changes in neuronal excitability,

74

up and down regulation of neurotransmitter receptors, synaptic plasticity and

75

neuronal growth (Smith and Curthoys 1989; Curthoys 2000; Dutia 2010). In

76

conjunction with the improvement of vestibular symptoms and reflexes, quality of

77

life also improves (Bergenius and Perols 1999; Kim et al, 2008; Patel et al, 2016;).

78 79

Despite these general trends, however, the degree of clinical recovery after a UVL is

80

variable and unpredictable in individual patients. This variability depends less on

81

the severity of the lesion, as measured with tests of vestibulo-ocular reflex (VOR)

82

function (degree of canal paresis (Shupak et al, 2008) or head-impulse test

83

abnormality; Palla et al, 2008, Patel et al, 2016), and more on individual differences

84

in neurological (Adamec et al, 2015), psychological (Godemann et al, 2004 ) and

85

visual psycho-physical factors (Cousins et al, 2014; 2017). However, the

86

dependence of recovery on vestibularly-mediated motion perception has been

87

neglected, despite animal physiology (Angelaki and Cullen 2008) and human

88

imaging studies (Dieterich 2007; Helmchen et al, 2009) showing significant

89

contributions by widespread cortico-subcortical structures to central vestibular

90

processing and lesion-induced neural plasticity.

91 92

Acute UVL are known to induce dissociation between vestibulo-perceptual and

93

vestibulo-ocular reflex function (Cousins et al, 2013) but, as central compensation

94

develops, these differences are reduced (Cousins et al, 2017). Recent experiments

95

however suggest that perceptual-reflex dissociations can be induced in normal

96

subjects exposed to asymmetric rotation (Panichi et al, 2011; Pettorossi et al, 2013,

97

Pettorossi and Schieppati 2015; Pettorossi et al, 2015). In the course of repetitive

3

98

asymmetric rotations, VOR responses keep or improve symmetry. Notably,

99

however, central adaptive mechanisms make self-motion perception progressively

100

more asymmetric. We postulate that activation of such central mechanisms

101

involved in asymmetric vestibular adaptation are likely to play a critical part during

102

the adaptive process of compensation after unilateral labyrinthine lesions. Thus,

103

biases in the motion perception system may persist longer than VOR biases during

104

the compensatory period and this perceptual error could be responsible for long-

105

term symptom development. This is the general hypothesis examined here.

106 107

Therefore, we investigated self-motion perception acutely and during

108

compensation after UVL by using an asymmetric rotation technique known to

109

induce a bias in central vestibular circuitry of normal subjects (Panichi et al, 2011).

110

We compared this with the responses to symmetric rotation because symmetric

111

sinusoidal rotation (perhaps due to additional motion predictive components) is

112

unlikely to reveal long-term biases in vestibular motion perception. We also

113

compared vestibulo-perceptual responses (ultimately cortically mediated) to the

114

VOR elicited with the same symmetric and asymmetric stimuli, and with

115

conventional clinical caloric, head shaking and subjective visual vertical testing.

116

Finally, to examine the hypothesis that abnormalities in vestibularly-mediated

117

motion perception may contribute to long-term dizziness, we assessed the patients’

118

subjective clinical recovery using the Dizziness Handicap Inventory (DHI; Jacobson

119

and Newman 1990; Nola et al, 2010).

4

120 121

Material and Methods

122

Participants

123

Thirty male patients aged 24-55 years (mean 42.80, SD 8.35) were enrolled in a

124

prospective study in UVL: 24 right-handed, 6 handed, 12 with vestibular deficit in

125

the left side and 18 in the right side. Thirty normal subjects (controls) were also

126

examined, age 20-55 years old, mean 39±11.3; 25 right-handed, 5 left-handed.

127

Since in our previous study on perceptual responses to asymmetric rotation in

128

normal subjects (Pettorossi et al, 2013) male participants showed less variability

129

than female, we examined only male in the present study. Patients were first seen

130

for acute dizziness between 1 January 2009 and 31 December 2013. Inclusion

131

criteria were based on signs and symptoms of vestibular neuritis described by

132

Strupp and Brandt (2009). No patient had additional auditory or CNS symptoms or

133

signs. They all received methylprednisolone for 15 days and were encouraged to

134

become progressively physically active as symptoms subsided. All patients

135

performed the standard vestibular rehabilitation program offered in our hospital.

136 137

The patients were examined four times: approximately at one week (acute phase)

138

and at 4, 8 and 12 months after the acute episode. The 30 control subjects were also

139

tested four times at the same time intervals. Subjects' consent and protocol

140

approval followed the Helsinki declaration (1964) and was approved by the ethics

141

committee of the University of Perugia. Testing was performed by a different team

142

to the one participating in data analysis, largely unaware of the research questions.

143 144

Test of self-motion perception: stimulation apparatus and recording

145

Stimulation apparatus. Subjects were seated on a rotating chair, within an

146

acoustically isolated cabin, that rotated in the horizontal plane driven by a DC

147

motor (Powertron, Contraves, Charlotte, NC, USA) servo-controlled by an angular-

148

velocity encoder (0.01.- 1 Hz, 1% accuracy). A head holder maintained the head 30°

149

down tilted to align the horizontal semicircular canals with the rotational plane of

5

150

the platform (Fig. 1). The trunk was tightly fastened to the chair. Roll and pitch

151

head displacements were prevented by a plastic collar.

152 153

The chair oscillated sinusoidally at an amplitude of 40°. The vestibular stimulus

154

used in the main experiment consisted of four cycles of asymmetric whole-body

155

oscillations in the dark with the same back and forth amplitude, but different

156

velocity (Pettorossi et al, 2013). The stimulus profile resulted from the combination

157

of two sinusoidal half-cycles of the same amplitude (40°) but different frequencies -

158

fast half cycle (Fast HC) = 0.38 Hz and slow half cycle (Slow HC) = 0.09 Hz (Fig. 1).

159

Peak acceleration during the fast half cycle was 120°/s2 with peak velocity 47°/s,

160

followed by the slow rotation in the opposite direction at a peak acceleration of

161

7°/s2, peak velocity 11°/s that returned the subject to the starting position. Both

162

peak acceleration and velocity values are above thresholds for vestibular activation

163

(Grabherr et al,2008, Seemungal et al, 2004, Valko et al, 2012). As a control

164

condition, prior to asymmetric stimulation, all subjects underwent symmetric

165

rotation (40°, 0.38 Hz and 0.09 Hz) in order to compare motion perception

166

responses to the two stimuli.

167 168

Self-motion perception recordings. We used a psychophysical tracking procedure to

169

assess self-motion perception (Siegle et al, 2009; Pettorossi et al, 2004). Before

170

starting the rotation, subjects fixated a target placed in front of them and were

171

asked to continue to imagine the same target throughout the rotation in the dark

172

with eyes closed. The target was a light spot (diameter 1 cm) projected onto the

173

wall of the dark cabin in front of the subject at 1.5 m from the eyes. The spot was

174

switched off just before rotation onset and switched back on after rotation had

175

ended. Subjects were instructed to continuously track the remembered spot in the

176

dark as if it was earth-fixed by counter-rotating the forearm partially flexed and

177

adducted to orient a hand pointer (Fig. 1) towards the remembered spot. The

178

pointer had a laser beam that was turned on at the end of rotation for measuring

179

the angular distance between the projection of the beam on the wall and the real

180

target position (final position error, FPE). All subjects reported that the procedure

6

181

felt simple and intuitive. It could be argued that the voluntary pointing task we

182

adopted may not truly reflect vestibularly-mediated self-motion perception.

183

However, similar pointing tasks using a vestibular-remembered saccade task

184

(Bloomberg et al, 1991 Nakamura and Bronstein, 1995) break down completely in

185

humans devoid of vestibular function. Indeed, the results presented here in

186

patients with unilateral vestibular lesions also indicate that the task is vestibularly

187

mediated.

188 189

During asymmetric rotation, subjects perceive the fast half cycle more vividly than

190

the slow half cycle (Pettorossi et al, 2013) and so, at the end of each session, the

191

target is erroneously represented in the direction of the slow half cycle (Fig.. 1).

192

This FPE results from the algebraic sum of single cycle errors plus additional

193

adaptation that further enhances the perception of the fast rotation and reduces

194

that of the slow rotation (Pettorossi et al, 2013). In order to assess these

195

mechanisms in UVL patients, we delivered two rounds of “four cycle asymmetric

196

stimulation” - one with the slow half cycle (Slow HC) towards the lesion side and

197

another session with the Slow HC towards the healthy side. These oppositely

198

directed asymmetrical rotations were randomly assigned in direction and

199

administered with an interval of 1 day. Since asymmetric rotation can induce long

200

lasting after-effects (Pettorossi et al, 2015; Pettorossi et al., 2013), we ascertained

201

in preliminary experiments in three UVL patients that 1day rest interval was

202

sufficient to prevent any carry over effects. The FPEs of the two “four cycle

203

rotation” were compared and the asymmetry index of the two FPEs for perceptual

204

responses was evaluated as follows: (FPE towards lesion - FPE away the lesion)

205

*100/the sum of the two FPEs.

206 207

For separately evaluating the perception to the slow and fast half cycles and

208

comparing with data from previous studies (Pettorossi et al., 2013) we recorded

209

both perceptual tracking and the VOR in 10 controls and 10 UVL patients chosen

210

for their superior tracking ability, as shown by the similarity of their tracking to the

211

shape of the stimulus, that is, without jerks and pauses. In these 10 patients and 10

7

212

control subjects, we continuously recorded on-line tracking by connecting the

213

chair-fixed pointer to a precision potentiometer (joystick, Fig. 1A), for detailed

214

analysis as in previous experiments (Pettorossi et al, 2013). Cumulative amplitude

215

of the motion perception during fast and slow half cycles were separately

216

computed by adding four half cycle responses of two rounds of “asymmetric

217

rotations”, with the Slow HC in opposite directions (i.e. towards or away from the

218

lesion). In these subjects the VOR was also recorded with bitemporal DC-EOG

219

(Pettorossi et al, 2013) in a separate session 1 day apart. Quick nystagmic

220

components were removed off-line and the cumulative amplitude of the four half

221

cycles slow phase eye movement (cumulative slow phase eye position, SPEP)

222

(Bloomberg et al, 1991; Popov et al, 1999) were computed for fast and slow

223

rotation. The asymmetry in the VOR (asymmetry index) was also evaluated after

224

two oppositely directed rounds of “4 cycle asymmetric rotations” with the formula:

225

(lesion-Slow HC cumulative SPEP - healthy-Slow HC cumulative SPEP ) *100/ the

226

sum of the two SPEPs. Signals from the pointer, EOG and chair motion were digitised

227

by a 12-bit A/D card (Labview, National Instrument, and USA) at a sampling rate of

228

500 Hz and stored for off-line analysis.

229 230

Conventional (clinical) Vestibular Tests

231

Caloric test. Irrigation was performed with the patient supine and the head raised

232

30° according to Fitzgerald-Hallpike method, with water at 44° C and subsequently

233

at 30° C for 40 seconds, five minutes apart. We measured peak slow phase eye

234

velocity 60-90 seconds after irrigation onset and applied Jongkees formula for

235

canal paresis [(right cold + right warm) – (left cold + left warm)] *100/ (right warm

236

+ left cold + left warm + right cold)] and for directional preponderance [(right warm

237

+ left cold) – (right cold + left warm) *100 /(right warm + left cold + left warm + right

238

cold)] (Jacobson et al, 1995, Jonkees et al, 1965).

239 240

Head shaking nystagmus test. With the patient seated and the head flexed 30°, the

241

head was passively rotated horizontally by +/-45° at 1 Hz for 20 seconds, followed

242

by EOG recording of any evoked nystagmus. The Head Shaking Test was considered

8

243

positive if the head shaking induced at least 2 clear post rotation nystagmic beats

244

with a peak slow phase eye velocity (SPEV) > 5 deg/s (Kamei et al, 1964).

245 246

Subjective visual vertical. We used a faintly illuminated LED bar which was placed in

247

a darkened room and set 1.50 m away, straight ahead from the seated subjects. The

248

bar (30 x 1 cm) was mounted on the wall via a central rod, around which the bar

249

could be rotated by remote control in both directions. At the upper end, there was a

250

pointer that slid with the bar on a graduated scale (Faralli et al, 2007); in which

251

0°corresponds to perfect alignment with gravity. The bar was presented tilted 45°

252

to the right or left and subjects realigned the bar to perceived verticality. Three

253

measurements per side were conducted alternating the starting position from 45°

254

right and left tilt. The mean value of the six measurements will be reported. In

255

accordance with the literature mean values of subjective visual vertical between

256

±2° were considered normal (Friedman 1970).

257 258

DHI. Clinical outcome at 12 months was assessed with the Dizziness Handicap

259

Inventory (DHI; Jacobson and Newman, 1990, Nola et al, 2010). The DHI comprises

260

25 items (questions) and three possible replies: “yes” (four points), “sometimes”

261

(two points) and “no” (zero points) designed to access a patient’s functional (nine

262

questions), emotional (nine questions) and physical (seven questions) limitations.

263

A score

Asymmetric vestibular stimulation reveals persistent disruption of motion perception in unilateral vestibular lesions.

Self-motion perception was studied in patients with unilateral vestibular lesions (UVL) due to acute vestibular neuritis at 1 week, 4, 8 and 12 months...
4MB Sizes 0 Downloads 11 Views