http://informahealthcare.com/ada ISSN: 0095-2990 (print), 1097-9891 (electronic) Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2014; 40(5): 359–366 ! 2014 Informa Healthcare USA, Inc. DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2014.910521

ORIGINAL ARTICLE

Association of acculturation with drinking games among Hispanic college students Seth J. Schwartz, PhD1, Byron L. Zamboanga, PhD2, Cara C. Tomaso2, Karli K. Kondo, PhD3, Jennifer B. Unger, PhD4, Robert S. Weisskirch, PhD5, Lindsay S. Ham, PhD6, Alan Meca, MS7, Miguel A´ngel Cano, PhD, MPH8, Susan Krauss Whitbourne, PhD9, Aerika S. Brittian, PhD10, Sabrina E. Des Rosiers, PhD11, Eric A. Hurley, PhD12, Alexander T. Vazsonyi, PhD13, and Russell D. Ravert, PhD14 1

Department of Public Health Sciences, University of Miami, 2Department of Psychology, Smith College, 3AHRQ Scientific Resource Center, Portland VA Evidence-Based Synthesis Program, 4Department of Preventive Medicine, University of Southern California, 5Department of Human Development, California State University, Monterey Bay, 6Department of Psychological Science, University of Arkansas, 7Department of Psychology, Florida International University, 8Department of Health Disparities Research, The University of Texas MD Anderson Cancer Center, 9Department of Psychology, University of Massachusetts-Amherst, 10Department of Educational Psychology, University of Illinois at Chicago, 11Department of Psychology, Barry University, 12Departments of Psychology and Africana Studies, Pomona College, 13Department of Family Studies, University of Kentucky, and 14Department of Human Development and Family Studies, University of Missouri-Columbia, USA

Abstract

Keywords

Objective: This cross-sectional study was conducted to investigate which components of acculturation relate to drinking games participation among Hispanic college students. We also sought to examine whether the relationships between acculturation and drinking games would differ from the associations between acculturation and other alcohol-related outcomes. Method: A sample of 1,397 Hispanic students aged 18–25 (75% women; 77% US-born) from 30 US colleges and universities completed a confidential online survey. Results: Associations among acculturative processes, drinking games participation, general alcohol consumption, and negative drinking consequences differed across gender. Most significant findings emerged in the domain of cultural practices. For women, US cultural practices were associated with greater general alcohol consumption, drinking games frequency, and amount of alcohol consumed while gaming, whereas for men, US cultural practices were associated with general alcohol consumption and negative drinking consequences. Conclusions: Hispanic and US cultural practices, values, and identifications were differentially associated with drinking games participation, and these associations differed by gender. It is therefore essential for college student alcohol research to examine US culture acquisition and Hispanic culture retention separately and within the domains of cultural practices, values, and identifications.

Acculturation, alcohol, college students, drinking games, hispanics, problem drinking

Introduction Many college students participate in drinking games, which are high-risk, social drinking activities that are designed to facilitate inebriation, consist of rules that indicate when players drink and the quantity of alcohol that they must consume, and involve performing motor and/or mental tasks (1). Studies conducted with large, multisite samples of college students report prevalence rates of drinking games participation ranging from 49% (2) to 70% (3). Studies also indicate that individuals who participate in drinking games are at risk for experiencing negative alcohol-related consequences (2,3).

Address correspondence to Seth J. Schwartz, PhD, Department of Public Health Sciences, Leonard M. Miller School of Medicine, University of Miami, 1120 N.W.14th Street, Suite 1073, Miami, FL 33136, USA. E-mail: [email protected]

History Received 18 November 2013 Revised 19 March 2014 Accepted 21 March 2014 Published online 27 August 2014

Although research examining the demographic, psychological, and event-specific factors that can place students at risk for heavy consumption and negative drinking consequences are plentiful in the college alcohol literature (4), most have been conducted with predominantly White samples. A few studies (5) have examined the cultural correlates of high-risk drinking behaviors among Hispanic college students, but to our knowledge, no published studies have examined cultural correlates of drinking games specifically. Hispanics are an important population to examine both because of their rapidly growing numbers and because of their increasing presence on college campuses (6,7). As the proportion of Hispanics on college campuses continues to increase, identifying and understanding correlates of health risk behaviors in this population will become an increasingly important public health issue. In terms of alcohol use among Hispanic students, Venegas et al. (5) found that 48% of the

360

S. J. Schwartz et al.

Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2014; 40(5): 359–366

Hispanic students in their sample reported binge drinking at least once per week. In comparison, the Substance Use and Mental Health Services Administration (SAMHSA) (8) reports that 40% of students (regardless of ethnicity) binge drink at least once per month. Hispanic students may therefore engage in greater amounts of heavy drinking compared to students from other ethnic groups.

Acculturation Among Hispanic college students, acculturation has often been identified as a cultural correlate of drinking behaviors (9–17). Acculturation is the process of psychological, social, and cultural change and adaptation that takes place when cultural minorities engage in extended close interactions with the majority culture (18). However, to our knowledge, no published work has examined the specific associations between acculturation and drinking games. In the present study, we examine how components of acculturation are related to Hispanic college students’ involvement in drinking games and other alcohol-related variables. We also examine whether the associations of acculturation components with drinking games differ from the relationships of acculturation with general alcohol consumption and negative drinking consequences. Dimensions, domains, and components of acculturation According to Schwartz et al.’s seminal model (18), acculturation can be described in terms of dimensions, domains, and components. Dimensions refer to US-culture acquisition and heritage-culture retention, which can be considered either as opposite ends of a single continuum (unidimensional) or as separate processes (bidimensional) (18). Unidimensional conceptualizations of acculturation propose that individuals forsake their heritage cultural practices, values, and identifications in favor of adopting the receiving culture’s practices, values, and identifications. Conversely, according to bidimensional conceptualizations, individuals can identify with the receiving society and acquire its values and cultural practices while also maintaining their heritage practices, values, and identity. For example, a person can be traditionally oriented at home but more ‘‘Americanized’’ at work or with friends. Indeed, bidimensional models of acculturation are not only preferred, but are more consistent with the ‘‘on the ground’’ reality that immigrants and their immediate descendants experience (19). However, many studies on acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students continue to utilize unidimensional models, despite researchers’ recommendations (20,21) and empirical support (22) to the contrary. Within each acculturation dimension, individuals may evidence changes in at least three domains: practices (heritage or US language use and cultural activities), values (individualism-collectivism), and identifications (ethnic identity/US identity). Practices represent a behavioral dimension of acculturation, focusing on specific customs reflective of heritage or US cultural streams. Values represent a cognitive dimension of acculturation, focusing on beliefs or norms associated with a given cultural context. Identifications represent an affective dimension of acculturation, focusing

Figure 1. Acculturation framework.

on the extent to which one feels attached to one’s heritage culture and to the United States (19). Combining the use of three acculturation domains with a bidimensional model yields six acculturation components: US practices, Hispanic practices, individualist values, collectivist values, ethnic identity, and US identity (see Figure 1). Empirical research (22) indicates that these components are modestly intercorrelated but are not redundant with one another. Within each dimension (US and Hispanic acculturation), identifications are significantly related to both practices and values, but practices and values are not related to one another. The six acculturation components are sufficiently independent to be included together as predictors of mental health and substance use outcomes (22,23). Acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students Most studies examining acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students have employed unidimensional acculturation models (17). Some studies have yielded positive associations between acculturation and drinking behaviors (10,13), whereas others have found inverse associations (16), and still another reported no relationship (12). Only a handful of alcohol use studies with Hispanic college students have utilized bidimensional conceptions of acculturation, and these results have also yielded mixed findings. For example, Des Rosiers et al. (11) used latent class modeling to create five acculturative orientations based on Berry’s (24) classic typology: assimilated (acquires US practices and discards one’s cultural heritage), separated (rejects US practices and retains one’s cultural heritage), and three bicultural groups that involve both adopting US practices and retaining those from one’s cultural heritage – separated bicultural, high bicultural, and low bicultural. Des Rosiers et al. (11) found that students who were in the three bicultural groups reported lower levels of drinking-related risk behaviors than students in the assimilated group. These findings suggest that acquiring US practices, while retaining one’s heritage-cultural practices, may be most protective against alcohol-related risks. Several recent studies have examined multiple domains of acculturation. Schwartz et al. (22) found no relationship between any domain of acculturation (i.e. values, identifications, and practices) and overall hazardous use among a large,

Acculturation and drinking games

DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2014.910521

multisite sample of first- and second-generation Hispanic college students. In a sample of recently immigrated Hispanic adolescents, Schwartz et al. (25) found that various dimensions of acculturation were related to two types of drinking behaviors – occasions of alcohol consumption and maximum drinks in a day – in different ways. For example, collectivist values and US identity were related to a lower maximum number of drinks, but not to frequency of drinking. Acculturation is a complex construct, and its relationships with drinking outcomes depend on the specific component(s) examined, as well as on the specific drinking behaviors in question. Drinking games participation represents a specific type of high-risk drinking behavior that, to our knowledge, has not been examined in relation to acculturation in prior published research. The extent to which it is important to pursue research examining the associations between acculturation and drinking games depends on how distinct these associations are from those between acculturation and other drinking behaviors. An even more complex picture emerges once gender is taken into consideration. Results from two studies using unidimensional models (13,14) indicate that, among women, linguistic acculturation to English is associated with increased levels of alcohol use. However, the opposite association emerged for men in one study (13), and no association was found for men in another (14). One short-term longitudinal study (15) examined whether US cultural practices predicted progression to problematic alcohol use over the first year of college among women but not men; however, no association emerged for either gender over this time period. Conversely, Ceballos et al. (9) found that, among Hispanic college women, frequency of drinking was positively associated with US orientation and negatively associated with Hispanic orientation; no such patterns emerged for men. It is possible that Hispanic college women are acculturating into a sociocultural environment that, compared to their heritage culture, has fewer proscriptions against women drinking alcohol (17). Indeed, Zamboanga et al. (16) found that a stronger ethnic identity (within the domain of identifications) was associated with increased frequency of heavy alcohol use for Hispanic college men, but not women. Because acculturation often occurs within a gendered US context, particularly with regard to the acceptability of alcohol use, acculturation might influence alcohol use in Hispanic college women differently than it influences alcohol use in men. However, the exact nature of gender differences remains unclear and may vary depending on the specific domains of acculturation considered and the type of drinking behavior examined. Given the lack of published literature on the associations between acculturation domains and drinking games participation, we focused on these relationships in the present study. Taken together, the extant literature (including studies using both unidimensional and bidimensional models) does not indicate clear or consistent patterns of associations between acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students, nor between men and women within this population (17). These discrepancies may be the result of differing operational definitions of acculturation across studies. The present study contributes to the literatures on acculturation

361

and alcohol use by examining multiple acculturation domains (e.g. collectivist values, US identifications, Hispanic practices) and investigating how these domains are associated with drinking games participation and other alcohol-related variables among Hispanic college students (17). Only a few studies have examined acculturation processes in multiple domains (e.g. practices, values, and identifications) as predictors of alcohol use. Zamboanga et al. (26) found that ethnic identity, but not Hispanic or US practices, was positively related to the extent of alcohol use among Hispanic adolescents. Schwartz et al. (25) found that, among recent Hispanic immigrant adolescents, collectivist values were negatively predictive, and that ethnic identity was positively predictive, of maximum number of drinks in a day. In a sample of Hispanic college students, Schwartz et al. (22) found that none of the acculturation components were related to hazardous drinking. The current study addressed the following research questions: (i) Are the associations between acculturation domains and drinking games participation similar for Hispanic men and women? and (ii) How might different domains of acculturation be associated with drinking games participation, and how might these relationships differ from those involving general alcohol consumption or negative drinking consequences? Given the mixed findings in the literature, any hypotheses that we advance would be tentative. However, given the differences between Hispanic and US cultural contexts in terms of alcohol use among women, we expected that, among women, greater endorsement of US orientations, and lower endorsement of Hispanic orientations, would be associated with higher levels of all alcohol-related outcomes (27).

Methods Participants The present sample consisted of 1,397 self-identified Hispanic college students (343 men, 1,054 women). Participants were recruited from 30 colleges and universities in 20 US states. Although participants as old as 45 years of age completed the survey, we restricted the analytic sample to individuals 18–25 years of age. Among participants who indicated their or their families’ countries of origin, the most common were Mexican (19%), Cuban (16%), Colombian (6%), and Dominican (4%). The study was approved by the Institutional Review Boards at the senior author’s home university and at each participating institution. Sites were selected in order to provide diversity in terms of geographic location, setting (urban, suburban, or college town), and type of institution. Six sites were located in the Northeast, seven in the Southeast, six in the Midwest, three in the Southwest, and eight in the West. Fifteen of the sites were major public universities, eight were smaller/commuter state universities, four were major private universities, and three were private colleges. The representation of Hispanics within the student body ranged from 1–60% (mean 12%). Procedures At each site, participants were directed to the study website using printed or emailed announcements. Participants were

362

S. J. Schwartz et al.

asked to read an online consent form and to check a box indicating that they agreed to participate. Participants completed the survey at their convenience. Only participant ID numbers were included in our data files, and these numbers were not linked with any identifying information. Students were asked to provide their email addresses and student ID numbers for purposes of providing course credit, but no identifying information was linked to participants’ responses. Students also provided the name of their college or university; however, this information was used only to control for multilevel nesting and was not used to identify specific participants or schools. Respondents received credit toward their course grades in exchange for their participation. Completion time for the entire survey ranged from 1–2 hours.

Measures1 Acculturation Acculturation was measured using three cultural domains: practices, values, and identifications. Cultural practices were assessed using the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (28). This measure consists of two subscales: heritage-culture practices (17 items, a ¼ 0.91; e.g. ‘‘I listen to music of my ethnic group’’), which includes use of one’s heritage language and association with heritage-culture friends and romantic partners; and US-culture practices (15 items, a ¼ 0.84; e.g. ‘‘I speak English at home’’), which includes use of English and association with US friends and romantic partners. In terms of cultural values, the dynamic between individualism and collectivism serves as a backdrop for the Hispanic immigrant experience, where US culture is largely individualist and many Hispanic cultures are largely collectivist (29). Accordingly, we operationalized cultural values in terms of individualism and collectivism. Individualism (a ¼ 0.76; e.g. ‘‘I’d rather depend on myself than on others’’) and collectivism (a ¼ 0.80; e.g. ‘‘It is my duty to take care of my family, even when I have to sacrifice what I want’’) were assessed using corresponding 8-item scales developed by Triandis and Gelfand (30). Cultural identifications were measured using the MultiGroup Ethnic Identity Measure (MEIM) (31) and the American Identity Measure (AIM) (32) to assess heritage and US identifications, respectively. The MEIM consists of 12 items (a ¼ 0.92) that assess the extent to which one (a) has considered the subjective meaning of one’s race/ethnicity (e.g. ‘‘I think a lot about how my life will be affected by my ethnic group membership’’) and (b) feels positively about one’s racial/ethnic group (e.g. ‘‘I am happy that I am a member of the ethnic group I belong to’’). The AIM (a ¼ 0.90) was developed by inserting ‘‘the United States’’ in place of ‘‘my ethnic group’’ in the MEIM. Drinking games participation We assessed drinking games participation using two items. We first asked about frequency of participation since the person had been in college using a response scale ranging 1

Unless otherwise specified, 5-point Likert scales were used for all study measures, with responses ranging from 1 (Strongly disagree) to 5 (Strongly agree).

Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2014; 40(5): 359–366

from 0 (I don’t play drinking games) to 7 (Nearly every day). We then asked about the usual number of drinks consumed while playing drinking games. Response choices ranged from 0 (I don’t play drinking games) to 5 (7 or more drinks). General alcohol consumption and negative drinking consequences We used the consumption subscale from the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT) (33) to index general consumption (34,35) (a ¼ 0.85). Specifically, participants were asked how often they consume alcoholic drinks, how many drinks containing alcohol they consume on a typical day of drinking, and how often they consume six or more drinks on a single occasion. The remaining seven AUDIT items were used to index negative drinking consequences (a ¼ 0.81; e.g. being unable to remember events that occurred while one was drinking, injuring someone because of one’s drinking). Data analytic plan Our data analytic plan consisted of four steps, the last two of which involved structural equation modeling (SEM) using Mplus (36). First, we examined missing data rates and tested whether data were missing completely at random. Second, we computed correlation coefficients among the acculturation variables and among the alcohol use variables. Third, we conducted a multigroup invariance test across gender (see Figure 2). Specifically, we tested whether three sets of paths differed between men and women: (a) from acculturation to drinking games participation and to usual number of drinks consumed while gaming, (b) from acculturation to general alcohol consumption, and (c) from acculturation to negative drinking consequences. Fourth, we estimated an SEM model where the six acculturation components (i.e. US practices, Hispanic practices, individualism, collectivism, US identity, Hispanic identity) were allowed to predict (a) frequency of gaming and the usual number of drinks consumed while gaming, (b) general alcohol consumption, and (c) negative drinking consequences. Depending on whether the model parameters were equivalent across gender, we planned to specify the model using the whole sample if the parameters were equivalent or in multigroup form if parameters were not equivalent.

Results We first examined rates of missing data and evaluated whether we could safely estimate missing values using maximum likelihood estimation. Missing data rates ranged from 2.5% (US practices) to 23.4% (drinks consumed while playing drinking games). We conducted Little’s Missing Completely at Random (MCAR) (37) test to ascertain whether data could be assumed to be MCAR. The test produced a non-significant result, 2 (77) ¼ 89.30, p ¼ 0.16, indicating that the MCAR assumption was met. As a result, missing data were handled using robust full information maximum likelihood (FIML) estimation (38). Table 1 presents means and percentages for study variables, Table 2 presents correlations among the various alcohol use variables, and Table 3 presents correlations among the acculturation components. All of the correlations

Acculturation and drinking games

DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2014.910521

363

Figure 2. Analytic model. Note that the two overlapping boxes represent a multigroup model estimated separately on men and on women.

Table 1. Descriptive statistics for study variables. Cronbach’s aa

Variable Acculturation US practices Hispanic practices Individualist values Collectivist values US identity Ethnic identity Drinking games frequency Never Less than once a month Once a month 2–3 times a month Once a week 2–3 times a week Daily or nearly daily Consumption while gamingc 1 Drink per occasion 2 Drinks per occasion 3–4 Drinks per occasion 5–6 Drinks per occasion 7 or more drinks per occasion

Men (M, SD)/%

0.84 0.91 0.76 0.80 0.90 0.92

62.22 54.40 29.86 30.83 46.73 42.96

(7.40) (14.47) (4.89) (4.65) (8.50) (10.06)

Women (M, SD)/% 62.29 56.19 28.51 31.01 45.73 43.46

(8.18) (15.37) (4.93) (4.90) (9.10) (10.17)

F/2

Z2

0.02 3.90* 18.87*** 0.34 3.11 0.62

0.00 0.00 0.01 0.00 0.00 0.00

46.3% 17.6% 11.0% 14.3% 5.1% 4.8% 0.3%

54.5% 20.4% 8.2% 8.2% 4.0% 3.9% 0.5%

4.36* 1.35 1.33 6.76* 0.44 0.26 0.22

N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab

5.5% 11.7% 15.4% 12.1% 9.5%

4.8% 10.6% 17.8% 9.3% 4.0%

0.10 0.07 1.25 1.07 10.76**

N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab N/Ab

Estimated on the full sample. bEffect sizes are not provided for one-sample Chi-square tests. cParticipants included those who reported that they play drinking games. *p50.05, **p50.01, ***p50.001.

a

Table 2. Associations among alcohol use variables. Variable 1. 2. 3. 4.

Drinking games frequency Consumption while gaming General alcohol consumption Negative drinking consequences

2

3

4

0.68/0.74 –

0.58/0.49 0.75/0.67 –

0.31/0.31 0.30/0.41 0.45/0.55 –

Within each cell, correlations for men are presented first, followed by correlations for women. All correlation coefficients are significant at p50.001.

among alcohol use variables were positive, moderate, or strong, and statistically significant for both men and women. Correlations among acculturation components were generally positive for both genders, especially those involving the domains of values and identifications.

Research Question 1: Consistency of associations across gender Results indicated that model parameters differed significantly across gender, D2(24) ¼ 60.42, p50.001. This finding indicates that the remaining analyses should be conducted separately for men and for women. Research Question 2: Associations of acculturation domains with drinking games involvement, and differences between these relationships and links of acculturation with other alcohol-related behaviors Two separate models were estimated: one including the entire sample and using drinking games frequency, general alcohol consumption, and drinking consequences; and a second model

364

S. J. Schwartz et al.

Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2014; 40(5): 359–366

Table 3. Bivariate correlations among acculturation components. Component US practices Individualist values US identity Hispanic practices Collectivist values Ethnic identity

US practices

Individualist values

US identity

Hispanic practices

Collectivist values

Ethnic identity

– 0.25*** 0.55*** 0.24*** 0.16*** 0.07*

0.23*** – 0.28*** 0.04 0.22*** 0.20***

0.39*** 0.19*** – 0.07* 0.34*** 0.21***

0.08 0.08 0.20*** – 0.23*** 0.50***

0.33*** 0.26*** 0.35*** 0.26*** – 0.43***

0.13* 0.23*** 0.36*** 0.61*** 0.52*** –

Correlations above the diagonal are for men, and correlations below the diagonal are for women. *p50.05, **p50.01, ***p50.001. Table 4. Path coefficients between acculturation and alcohol use variablesa.

Acculturation component US practices Men Women Hispanic practices Men Women Individualist values Men Women Collectivist values Men Women US identity Men Women Ethnic identity Men Women

Drinking games frequency

Consumption while gaming

0.10 0.14**

General alcohol consumption

0.08 0.10**

Negative drinking consequences

0.12** 0.11*

0.18*** 0.05

0.06 0.08x

0.12x 0.07*

0.07 0.07

0.05 0.08*

0.07 0.05

0.18*** 0.01

0.06 0.01

0.03 0.01

0.03 0.02

0.13* 0.04

0.16** 0.09

0.10 0.13**

0.00 0.00

0.01 0.01

0.11 0.04

0.05 0.06

0.01 0.00

0.08 0.04

0.04 0.05

0.02 0.05

a

Values presented are standardized regression coefficients, controlling for the other acculturation components as well as for age and annual family income. xp50.06, *p50.05, **p50.01, ***p50.001.

that used only participants who reported gaming at least once per month. The latter model included the amount of alcohol consumed (see Table 4) while gaming as an outcome. Both models were estimated in multigroup form across gender, and age and income were included as covariates for all outcomes. Both models fit the data well: both gamers and non-gamers, 2(14) ¼ 15.75, p ¼ 0.33; CFI ¼ 0.999; NNFI ¼ 0.999; RMSEA ¼ 0.016 (90% CI ¼ 0.000–0.048); SRMR ¼ 0.016; gamers only, 2(14) ¼ 15.69, p ¼ 0.33; CFI ¼ 0.981; NNFI ¼ 0.960; RMSEA ¼ 0.016 (90% CI ¼ 0.000–0.048); SRMR ¼ 0.018. Drinking games frequency For men, there were no significant predictors of gaming frequency. For women, US practices, b ¼ 0.14, p50.01, and Hispanic practices, b ¼ 0.08, p50.06, emerged as predictors of gaming frequency. These results indicate that greater endorsement of US practices, and lower endorsement of Hispanic practices, are associated with more frequent engagement in drinking games among women. Consumption while gaming Again, analyses for consumption while gaming were conducted using only participants who reported gaming at least once per month. For men, individualist values, b ¼ 0.18, p50.001, emerged as the only significant correlate. For

women, US practices, b ¼ 0.10, p50.01, and Hispanic practices, b ¼ 0.07, p50.05 emerged as significant or marginally significant correlates. General alcohol consumption Among men, US practices, b ¼ 0.12, p50.01, and US identity, b ¼ 0.11, p50.01, both emerged as positive correlates. For women, US practices, b ¼ 0.11, p50.05, emerged as the only significant correlate. Negative drinking consequences US practices, b ¼ 0.18, p50.001, emerged as the only significant predictors among men. For women, Hispanic practices, b ¼ 0.08, p50.03; and US identity, b ¼ 0.13, p50.01, emerged as significant correlates.

Discussion This study examined the associations between acculturation components and drinking games involvement among Hispanic college students. We also investigated how certain cultural domains of acculturation are differentially associated with drinking games participation and other alcohol-related variables. With regard to drinking games, the most consistent set of correlates were in the practices domain, especially for women. US practices (positively) and Hispanic practices (negatively) were associated with both drinking games frequency and

Acculturation and drinking games

DOI: 10.3109/00952990.2014.910521

consumption while gaming. These findings are consistent with prior research suggesting that acculturation (as indexed by cultural practices; e.g. English language use) is positively associated with increased levels of alcohol consumption among Hispanic college women (13,14). Among women, endorsement of US practices, along with fewer Hispanic practices, may signify a departure from traditional Hispanic drinking norms (27). For men, there was a positive association between individualistic values and increased levels of consumption while gaming. Perhaps lower levels of individualistic values denote lower orientations toward risk taking behavior, particularly in a high-risk context such as drinking games. With regard to the other alcohol-related behaviors, much like the pattern observed among women, US practices were positively related to general alcohol consumption among men; however, US identification was positively related to general consumption for men only. These findings are contrary to another study (14) that utilized language use to index acculturation and found no associations between acculturation and heavy use among Hispanic men. Differences in measurement of acculturation and alcohol use may help explain these discrepant findings in the present study and in the literature at large (17). Collectivist values were positively associated with negative drinking consequences for men, whereas for women, more US identification was associated with increased levels of negative drinking consequences. For men, more US practices were associated with lower levels of negative drinking consequences. Although Orona et al. (12) found no associations between acculturation (as indexed by practices) and drinking behaviors, the present results paint a different picture. Among women, identifying with the United States (along with greater endorsement of US practices) may represent a break from traditional gender norms, which in turn may be linked with greater likelihood of alcohol-related problems. Such gender imbalances do not exist for men, perhaps explaining why US practices were not linked with alcohol-related behaviors. These findings also shed light on the different patterns of associations that emerge between value- and identification-focused cultural dimensions of acculturation and drinking behaviors among Hispanic college men and women. The present findings are consistent with prior work (22) in stressing that different acculturation domains can have very different relationships to drinking outcomes. In terms of acculturation and drinking games research, our results underscore the importance of considering frequency of drinking games participation separately from consumption while gaming, a point that has been noted by Zamboanga et al. (39). They found that some students do not play drinking games at all, while some play drinking games frequently but do not consume heavy amounts of alcohol during each game. Others play infrequently but drink heavily when they do, and still others play often and consume a large number of drinks when playing. Based on the present findings, acculturation may play an important role in determining which of these profiles a specific student most closely resembles. The intervention implications (including interventions to facilitate heritage-culture retention) likely depend on a given student’s frequency of gaming and on the amount of alcohol consumed on each gaming occasion.

365

Limitations and future directions The present findings should be interpreted in the context of several important limitations. First, the cross-sectional design does not permit strong tests of directionality. Longitudinal designs, where prior values of the outcome variables are controlled, are needed to most definitively establish directionality in developmental research (40). Second, the present sample was 75% female, which is typical of social-science course enrollments; future studies could oversample men to generate a more gender-balanced sample. Third, because individuals often under-report or over-report risky behaviors for various reasons, collateral reports of drinking behavior and other risk-taking activities would be useful to collect in future studies. Fourth, individualism and collectivism were the only cultural values examined in the study. Other Hispanic cultural values, such as marianismo and machismo, which are central to Hispanic gender role socialization (41), may provide more insight into explaining the gender differences found in alcohol consumption. Fifth, the predominance of women in the present sample provides more statistical power to examine findings for women – although it should be noted that there was a reasonable number of men in the sample (n ¼ 343). Sixth, we did not include other important predictors of heavy alcohol use, such as parental alcoholism, antisociality, and Greek affiliation (42,43). Seventh, it is important to note the within-group diversity among Hispanics, who vary not just in terms of heritage nationality but also with regard to socioeconomic status, history in the United States, and other factors (6). Future studies should oversample from smaller Hispanic groups so that groupspecific analyses can be conducted. Finally, we acknowledge that the strengths of the regression coefficients reported in the present study are modest. In conclusion, despite these limitations, the present results indicate that certain acculturation domains are differentially associated with drinking games and other alcohol-related behaviors among Hispanic college men and women. Acculturation and drinking games participation are multifaceted constructs, and a more nuanced understanding of each construct is essential if we are to intervene in order to prevent or reduce problematic drinking among Hispanic students. Given the rapid expansion of both the Hispanic college student population and the Hispanic population as a whole, it is essential that more research on acculturation and drinking behaviors among Hispanic college students be conducted to understand which cultural domains pose a risk and which ones are protective. Much more work is needed to advance our understanding of the association between acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students, and the present study is only one of many important steps in this direction.

Acknowledgments This study was conduced as part of the Multi-Site University Study of Identity and Culture (MUSIC). All MUSIC collaborators are gratefully acknowledged. We thank Michael Vernon for managing the study website and all of the participants for their time and effort.

366

S. J. Schwartz et al.

Declaration of interest The authors report no conflicts of interest. The authors alone are responsible for the content and writing of this paper.

References 1. Zamboanga BL, Pearce MW, Kenney SR, Ham LS, Woods OE, Borsari B. Are ‘‘extreme consumption games’’ drinking games? Sometimes it’s a matter of perspective. Am J Drug Alc Abuse 2013; 39:275–279. 2. Grossbard J, Geisner IR, Neighbors C, Kilmer JR, Larimer ME. Are drinking games sports? College athlete participation in drinking games and alcohol-related problems. J Stud Alcohol Drugs 2007;68:97–105. 3. LaBrie J, Ehret P, Hummer J. Are they all the same? An exploratory, categorical analysis of drinking game types. Addict Behav 2013;38:2133–2139. 4. Mallett KA, Varvil-Weld L, Borsari B, Read JP, Neighbors C, White HR. An update of research examining college student alcohol-related consequences: new perspectives and implications for interventions. Alcsm Clin Exp Res 2013;37:709–716. 5. Venegas J, Cooper TV, Naylor N, Hanson BS, Blow JA. Potential cultural predictors of heavy episodic drinking in Hispanic college students. Am J Addict 2012;21:145–149. 6. Ennis SR, Rios-Vargas M, Albert NG. The Hispanic population, 2010 (Census brief C2010BR-4). Washington, DC: U.S. Census Bureau; 2011. 7. Fry R, Lopez MH. Hispanic enrollments reach new heights in 2011. Washington, DC: Pew Hispanic Center; 2012. 8. Substance Use and Mental Health Services Administration. Underage alcohol use among full-time college students. The NSDUH Report; Issue 31, 2006. 9. Ceballos NA, Czyzewska M, Croyle K. College drinking among Latinos(as) in the United States and Mexico. Am J Addict 2012;21: 544–549. 10. Despues D, Friedman HS. Ethnic differences in health behaviors among college students. J Appl Soc Psychol 2007;37:131–142. 11. Des Rosiers SE, Schwartz SJ, Zamboanga BL, Ham LS, Huang S. A cultural and social cognitive model of differences in acculturation orientations, alcohol expectancies, and alcohol-related risk behaviors among Hispanic college students. J Clin Psychol 2013; 69:319–340. 12. Orona JA, Blume AW, Morera OF, Perez S. Examining drinking consequences and reasons for drinking in a bilingual college sample. Hispanic J Behav Sci 2007;29:101–115. 13. Raffaelli M, Torres Stone RA, Iturbide MI, McGinley M, Carlo G, Crockett LJ. Acculturation, gender, and alcohol use among Mexican American college students. Addict Behav 2007;32:2187–2199. 14. Safer AM, Piane G. Analysis of acculturation, sex, and heavy alcohol use in Latino college students. Psychol Rep 2007;101: 565–573. 15. Schweitzer AC, Doran N, Roesch SC, Myers MG. Progression to problem drinking among Mexican American and White European first-year college students: a multiple group analysis. J Stud Alcohol Drug 2011;72:975–980. 16. Zamboanga BL, Raffaelli M, Horton NJ. Acculturation status and heavy alcohol use among Mexican American college students: investigating the moderating role of gender. Addict Behav 2006;31: 2188–2198. 17. Zamboanga BL, Tomaso CC, Kondo KK, Schwartz SJ. Surveying the literature on acculturation and alcohol use among Hispanic college students: we’re not all on the same page. Sub Use and Misuse 2014; 49:1074–1078. 18. Schwartz SJ, Unger JB, Zamboanga BL, Szapocznik J. Rethinking the concept of acculturation: implications for theory and research. Am Psychol 2010;65:237–251. 19. Arends-Toth J, van de Vijver FJR. Assessment of psychological acculturation. In: Berry JW, Sam DL, eds. The Cambridge handbook of acculturation psychology. Cambridge, UK: Cambridge University Press, 2006:142–160. 20. Castillo LG, Caver K. Expanding the concept of acculturation in Mexican American rehabilitation psychology research and practice. Rehabil Psychol 2009;54:351–362.

Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse, 2014; 40(5): 359–366

21. Lopez-Class M, Castro FG, Ramirez AG. Conceptions of acculturation: a review and statement of critical issues. Soc Sci Med 2011; 72:1555–1562. 22. Schwartz SJ, Waterman AS, Uman˜a-Taylor AJ, Lee RM, Kim SY, Vazsonyi AT, Huynh QL, et al. Dimensions of acculturation: associations with health risk behaviors among college students from immigrant families. J Couns Psychol 2011;58:27–41. 23. Schwartz SJ, Waterman AS, Uman˜a-Taylor AJ, et al. Acculturation and well-being among college students from immigrant families. J Clin Psychol 2013;69:298–318. 24. Berry JW. Immigration, acculturation, and adaptation. Appl Psychol Internat Rev 1997;46:5–34. 25. Schwartz SJ, Unger JB, Des Rosiers SE, Lorenzo-Blanco EI, Zamboanga BL, Huang S, Baezconde-Garbanati L, et al. Domains of acculturation and their effects on substance use and sexual behavior in recent Hispanic immigrant adolescents. Prev Sci 2014;15:385–396. 26. Zamboanga BL, Schwartz SJ, Jarvis LH, Van Tyne K. Acculturation and substance use among Hispanic early adolescents: investigating the mediating roles of acculturative stress and selfesteem. J Primary Prevent 2009;30:315–333. 27. Zemore SE. Re-examining whether and why acculturation relates to drinking outcomes in a rigorous, national survey of Latinos. Alcohol Clin Exp Res 2005;29:2144–2153. 28. Stephenson M. Development and validation of the Stephenson Multigroup Acculturation Scale (SMAS). Psychol Assess 2000;12: 77–88. 29. Hofstede G. Culture’s consequences: comparing values, behaviors, institutions and organizations across nations. Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 2001. 30. Triandis HC, Gelfand MJ. Converging measurement of horizontal and vertical individualism and collectivism. J Pers Soc Psychol 1998;74:118–128. 31. Phinney JS. The multigroup ethnic identity measure: a new scale for use with diverse groups. J Adolesc Res 1992;7:156–176. 32. Schwartz SJ, Park IJK, Huynh QL, Zamboanga BL, Uman˜a-Taylor AJ, Lee RM, Rodriguez L, et al. The American Identity Measure: development and validation across ethnic group and immigrant generation. Identity: Int J Theory Res 2012;12:93–128. 33. Saunders JB, Aasland OG, Babor TF, de la Fuente JR, Grant M. Development of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test (AUDIT): WHO collaborative project on early detection of persons with harmful alcohol consumption – II. Addiction 1993;88: 791–804. 34. Shields AL, Guttmannova K, Caruso, JC. An examination of the factor structure of the Alcohol Use Disorders Identification Test in two high-risk samples. Subst Use Misuse 2004;39:1161–1182. 35. Zamboanga BL, Casner HG, Borsari B, Olthuis JV, Ham LS, Schwartz SJ, Van Tyne K, et al. Knowing where they’re going: destination-specific pregaming behaviors in a multiethnic sample of college students. J Clin Psychol 2013;69:383–396. 36. Muthe´n LK, Muthe´n BO. Mplus user’s guide, version 6.0. Los Angeles, CA: Muthe´n and Muthe´n. 37. Little RJA. A test of missing completely at random for multivariate data with missing values. J Amer Stat Assoc 1988;83:1198–1202. 38. Enders CK. Applied missing data analysis. New York: The Guilford Press; 2010. 39. Zamboanga BL, Schwartz SJ, Van Tyne K, Ham LS, Olthuis JV, Huang S, Kim SY, et al. Drinking game behaviors among college students: how often and how much? Am J Drug Alcohol Abuse 2010;36:175–179. 40. Little TD. Longitudinal structural equation modeling. New York, NY: The Guilford Press; 2013. 41. Castillo LG, Perez FV, Castillo R, Ghosheh MR. Construction and initial validation of the Marianismo Beliefs Scale. Couns Psychol Quart 2010;23:163–175. 42. Iwamoto DK, Takamatsu S, Castellanos J. Binge drinking and alcohol-related problems among U.S.-born Asian Americans. Cult Divers Ethn Minor Psychol 2012;18:219–227. 43. Hussong AM, Huang W, Serrano D, Curran PJ, Chassin L. Testing whether and when parent alcoholism uniquely affects various forms of adolescent substance use. J Abnorm Child Psychol 2012;40: 1265–1276.

Copyright of American Journal of Drug & Alcohol Abuse is the property of Taylor & Francis Ltd and its content may not be copied or emailed to multiple sites or posted to a listserv without the copyright holder's express written permission. However, users may print, download, or email articles for individual use.

Association of acculturation with drinking games among Hispanic college students.

This cross-sectional study was conducted to investigate which components of acculturation relate to drinking games participation among Hispanic colleg...
451KB Sizes 2 Downloads 10 Views