BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49 DOI 10.1007/s40614-014-0017-y O R I G I N A L A RT I C L E

A New Frontier: Integrating Behavioral and Digital Technology to Promote Health Behavior Jesse Dallery 1 & Allison Kurti 1 & Philip Erb 1

Published online: 23 August 2014 # Association for Behavior Analysis International 2014

Abstract Modifiable behavioral risk factors such as cigarette smoking, physical inactivity, and obesity contribute to over 40 % of premature deaths in the USA. Advances in digital and information technology are creating unprecedented opportunities for behavior analysts to assess and modify these risk factors. Technological advances include mobile devices, wearable sensors, biomarker detectors, and real-time access to therapeutic support via information technology. Integrating these advances with behavioral technology in the form of conceptually systematic principles and procedures could usher in a new generation of effective and scalable behavioral interventions targeting health behavior. In this selective review of the literature, we discuss how technological tools can assess and modify a range of antecedents and consequences of healthy and unhealthy behavior. We also describe practical, methodological, and conceptual advantages for behavior analysts that stem from the use of technology to assess and treat health behavior. Keywords Behavioral assessment . Contingency management . Health behavior . mHealth . Technology Advances in technology are creating unprecedented opportunities for behavior analysts to assess and promote health behavior. Technological advances include mobile devices, wearable sensors, biomarker detectors, and real-time access to therapeutic interventions via information technology. In light of these innovations, Asch et al. (2012) have called for a new health-care model that enables “automated hovering,” or real-time monitoring, of patients’ behavior while they engage in everyday activities. Because most choices about health are made during everyday activities, influencing these choices constitutes the “single greatest opportunity to improve health and reduce premature

* Jesse Dallery [email protected] 1

Department of Psychology, University of Florida, P. O. Box 112250, Gainesville, FL 32611, USA

20

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

deaths” in the USA (Schroeder 2007, p. 1222). Even chronically ill patients spend only a few hours per year with a doctor or nurse, leaving 5,000 h per year in which they are engaged in other health-related behavior, including “deciding whether to take prescribed medications or follow other medical advice, deciding what to eat and drink and whether to smoke, and making other choices about activities that can profoundly influence their health” (Asch et al. 2012, p. 1). Technology is already in place to perform some forms of automated hovering of health behavior. There are over 1.5 billion Internet users and 5.6 billion cell phone users worldwide (International Telecommunications Union 2011). In the USA, 76 % of the population has home access to the Internet and 91 % owns at least one mobile phone (Brennan 2013). The prevalence of information technology suggests that most humans are already connected to a potential intervention device throughout most of the day. The ubiquity of technology, however, is just one part of the equation to promote behavior change. The second part is a science of behavior to inform developers and researchers about how technology can change behavior. In a major review of mobile health interventions, also known as mHealth, Kaplan and Stone (2013) noted that “many application developers seem unaware that there is a basic science of behavior change” (p. 491). In this article, we discuss how behavior analysis can inform technology-based assessment and treatment of health behavior. We hope to appeal to two audiences: behavior analysts interested in applying technology to health behavior and developers and researchers in the field of health behavior interested in harnessing behavioral principles to change behavior. First, we introduce a behavior-analytic conceptual framework. Second, we discuss innovative applications of technology to assess and change health-related behavior. Rather than performing a comprehensive review, we focus on illustrative developments to inform future intra- and interdisciplinary work in behavior analysis and allied fields. We also restrict our discussion to technological developments that have appeared in the scientific literature, as opposed to the vast array of largely untested consumer-oriented technologies and web sites. Third, we describe several practical, methodological, and conceptual advantages for the field of behavior analysis that stem from the use of technology.

Technology and Health Behavior: a Conceptual Framework The Four-Term Functional Unit of Analysis Most health-related behavior can be conceptualized as operant behavior. Operant behavior is any activity influenced by its consequences. Physical activity, eating fruit, taking a medication, and following a medical regimen are operant behavior because their consequences influence their future probability. Similarly, unhealthy behavior such as being sedentary, eating high fat foods, seeking illicit drugs, and refusing a medication are also examples of operant behavior because such behavior is maintained by its consequences. Once these activities are recognized as such, it follows that technology-based methods to change them will need to change one or more components of the functional unit of analysis. The functional unit consists of four components and their dynamic interaction: (a) motivating operations, (b) discriminative stimuli, (c) operant responses, and (d) consequences (Glenn et al. 1992; Thompson 2007). Figure 1

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

21

Motivating Operations Establishing Operations Abolishing Operations

Discriminative Stimuli

Operant Behavior

Consequences

Nonverbal discriminative stimuli (locations, people) Verbal discriminative stimuli (prompts, rules)

Healthy behavior Unhealthy behavior

Positive reinforcement Negative reinforcement Positive Punishment Negative Punishment

Fig. 1 The four-term functional unit of analysis

presents a schematic of the four-term unit. These components are relevant to health behavior interventions that assess and introduce antecedents and consequences to change unhealthy behavior, which constitute the majority of technology-based interventions (Kaplan and Stone 2013; Riley et al. 2011; Tomlinson et al. 2013). Motivating operations (MOs) can increase or decrease the efficacy of consequences as reinforcers or punishers (Laraway et al. 2003; Michael 2000; Sundberg 2013; for an MO account of eating behavior, see Tapper 2005; for an MO account of side effects of medications, see Valdovinos and Kennedy 2004). Events that increase the efficacy of consequences are establishing operations. Deprivation from cigarettes in a dependent smoker increases the efficacy of cigarettes as a reinforcer (a value-altering effect). An establishing MO will also evoke behaviors that have led to the reinforcer in the past, such as searching for a pack of cigarettes (a behavior-altering effect). Other examples of events that function as establishing operations include conditions of deprivation (from food, sex, sleep, social interaction, drugs of abuse), aversive stimulation (e.g., a headache, illness, heartburn), and the presence of illicit and licit drugs of abuse (e.g., marijuana may increase the value of food and evoke behavior that has led to food in the past). An MO can also decrease the efficacy of consequences, in which case the event is an abolishing operation (e.g., drinking a large volume of soda reduces the efficacy of soda as a reinforcer). In some cases, events may function as both establishing and abolishing operations. For example, the environmental conditions that lead to “stress” may enhance the value of certain activities such as sleep, drug use, or watching television, and simultaneously weaken others such as physical activity, socializing, and work-related tasks (Anderson et al. 2000; Dougher and Hackbert 2000). Conceptually, the physiological correlates, or endogenous events, associated with stress are concordant with the function of MOs (Thompson 2007). This is relevant because many engineers and health behavior scientists are using wearable sensors to detect endogenous events involving the autonomic nervous system (Boyer et al. 2012; Burns et al. 2011; Ertin et al. 2011; Plarre et al. 2011; Sun et al. 2012). The hope is that detecting endogenous events will help illuminate patterns between endogenous events and subsequent unhealthy behavior, which could lead to interventions to prevent the occurrence of unhealthy behavior.

22

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

Discriminative stimuli occasion responses, or make them more likely, by virtue of a differential history of reinforcement in the presence versus absence of these stimuli. Assuming that skipping a medication or binge eating, for instance, is maintained by attention from particular family members, the presence of those family members may serve as discriminative stimuli and make these responses more likely. In addition to stimuli that may occasion unhealthy behavior in naturalistic contexts, one purpose of technology-delivered stimuli is to serve as verbal discriminative stimuli 1 to promote health behavior (Hayes et al. 1986). Verbal discriminative stimuli to promote health behavior may be called prompts, cues, or reminders. If these messages occasion the relevant behavior (e.g., exercise, medication adherence, abstaining from smoking), the behavior is considered an instance of rule-following. The consequences for rulefollowing may be social (in which case the rule is called a ply, and the behavior is called pliance), or they may be embedded in the natural consequences for following a rule (in which case the rule is called a track, and the behavior is called tracking; Hayes et al. 1986). A text message to take a medication may serve as a ply or a track, depending on the reinforcing consequences, for example, avoiding a scolding parent (ply) or attenuating a headache (track). Alternatively, a technology-based message may occasion behavior and require no explicit reinforcement due to a long history of reinforcement for similar rule-following behavior, which then generalizes to the novel technology-based stimulus (i.e., generalized rule-following; Palmer 2012). Establishing operations such as food or drug deprivation can also enhance and broaden discriminative control (Lotfizadeh et al. 2012). In their review of the literature, Lotfizadeh et al. argued that establishing operations can increase not only the evocative strength of established discriminative stimuli but also of stimuli that are similar to the established discriminative stimuli. These findings may be relevant to understanding maintenance and relapse to substance use and other unhealthy behavior. Specifically, abstinence may increase stimulus control by already established stimuli such as particular people or settings, and this control may also generalize across a range of “untrained” stimuli. If drinking alcohol occurs in familiar bars, a lapse following abstinence may be occasioned by an unfamiliar restaurant or residence. This process should also apply to other health behavior where deprivation plays a role (e.g. eating, illicit drug use). There are two main approaches to addressing the consequences associated with health behavior. The first approach, which is the most common approach in applied behavior analysis, is to identify the consequences that maintain unhealthy behavior and then eliminate or attenuate their influence on behavior (Beavers et al. 2013; Hanley 2012; Iwata et al. 1994). The assumption, born out by hundreds of studies, is that unhealthy behavior serves a function, or multiple functions, for the individual. Speaking loosely, the behavior serves a purpose (e.g., social attention, automatic reinforcement, escape or attenuation of an unpleasant event). A clinician using a function-based approach would assess and then eliminate or modify the consequences of unhealthy behavior, or teach new, healthy behavior that serves the same function as the unhealthy behavior. A function-based account can also be applied to behavior that occurs infrequently. For example, behavior such as medication taking or physical activity 1 Distinguishing between the discriminative, motivative, and other functions of verbal stimuli is beyond the scope of this paper; for a discussion of alternative functions of verbal stimuli, see Schlinger (1993).

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

23

may occur at a low rate or not at all because they are punished by aversive consequences such as side effects or physical discomfort. The second approach, which is the most common in the field of health behavior, is to introduce immediate, desirable consequences based on evidence of health behavior (Higgins et al. 2008). Behavioral theories of choice and behavioral economics posit that choice for one of two options is influenced by the relative reinforcing value associated with each option (Bickel et al. 1993; Bigelow et al. 1998; Heyman 2009; Silverman 2004). Thus, choices to abstain or use drugs, eat healthy food or unhealthy food, or take or skip a medication can be influenced by introducing immediate, desirable consequences for the healthy choices. Immediate consequences may be critical given that many of the consequences for healthy choices are intermittent and delayed (e.g., social consequences for weight loss). Some potential reinforcers in the case of technologydelivered stimuli are social and monetary consequences (Alessi and Petry 2013; Dallery and Raiff 2011; Higgins et al. 2008), points, and game-based stimuli (Hackenberg 2009; Morford et al. 2014; Raiff et al. 2012).

Technology-Based Assessment Below we discuss technology-based assessment of antecedents, health behavior, and consequences. We have collapsed motivating operations and discriminative stimuli into one category labeled antecedents. In part, this is for convenience, but also because an antecedent could have motivative or discriminative functions, respondent functions, or no function for a particular individual. For example, the presence of friends could function as a discriminative stimulus or as an establishing operation depending on the individual’s history with respect to his or her friends and the behavior in question. Conceptual and empirical analysis should clarify whether the event signals the differential availability of a reinforcer (discriminative stimulus) or whether the event affects the differential value of an event as a reinforcer (motivating operation), or whether the antecedent functions in ways that we have not addressed in our basic outline of the four-term unit of analysis. Assessing Antecedents with Active Input Using Handheld Computers or Smartphones Ecological momentary assessment (EMA) is the most common method to conduct technology-based assessment of antecedents (Shiffman 2009a; Shiffman et al. 2008). Most applications of EMA use handheld computers or smart phones, and end users input information based on prompts at various times or based on events such as smoking a cigarette (Shiffman et al. 2008). EMA has been used to assess antecedent events associated with behavior such as alcohol use (Todd et al. 2009), illicit drug use (Buckner et al. 2012; Marhe et al. 2013; Shiffman 2009a, b), self-injury (Anestis et al. 2012; Armey et al. 2011), and behavior associated with mood and anxiety disorders (Forbes et al. 2012). Typically, EMA (a) occurs in naturalistic settings, (b) focuses on the participant’s internal experiences and antecedent situations, (c) samples behavior over long durations (e.g., weeks), and (d) occurs frequently (e.g., several times per day). EMA can therefore reveal behavioral variation over time and across contexts.

24

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

Shiffman et al. note that EMA methods are well-suited to health-related behavior because it occurs repeatedly and usually in discrete bouts, thus enabling “prospective assessments of the flow of behavior and experience, and of the antecedents and consequences of events…” (p. 10). Although a thorough review of EMA is beyond the scope of this article, research using EMA suggests that certain situations are more reliably associated with health behavior than others (Shiffman et al. 2008). In one relatively straightforward example, Mulvaney et al. (2012) tested the feasibility of using mobile phone-based EMA to assess adherence to diabetes self-management (e.g., glucose testing, insulin administration) in adolescents with type 1 diabetes. They found that the method was feasible, and moreover, that for a subset of adolescents, diabetes management was particularly challenging in the morning. This narrowing of the time window for deficient management could lead to further assessment and more targeted and efficient interventions. In general, compliance with completing EMA assessments is good, particularly when the assessment is prompted by the electronic device (Shiffman et al. 2008). For example, several studies report over 90 % compliance with prompted EMA (e.g., Cain et al. 2009). High rates are not observed universally, however, and recent research has examined the conditions that may influence compliance. Sokolovsky et al. (2012) identified several predictors of compliance in a sample of adolescents. Five to seven times per day for 7 days, adolescents were prompted to report on social context, location, and mood. On average, adolescents completed 67.5 % of the 14,105 prompted EMAs. Sokolovsky et al. found that greater negative affect, cigarette smoking rate, alcohol use, and male gender predicted lower compliance with random EMA prompts. When analysis focused on predicting compliance from one completed prompt to the next prompt, the authors found that increased positive affect, being outside of the home, and longer interprompt interval predicted lower compliance. Understanding sources of potential bias in EMA recording will be critical if researchers are to use EMA to assess the potential antecedents of unhealthy behavior. In addition, the validity of EMA in detecting antecedents will require further research. To our knowledge, no study has compared EMA to direct observation methods (or another gold standard) of antecedents of health behavior (for a discussion of direct observation, see Thompson and Borrero 2011). However, because many antecedents assessed with EMA consist of reports of endogenous events, direct observation methods may have limited utility in this domain. Although behavior analysts may be understandably skeptical about self-report methods, EMA offers advantages relative to traditional, questionnaire-based methods that occur at one or two time points during a study (e.g., real-time, frequent, longitudinal assessment in naturalistic settings). As such, EMA may be a valid and useful transducer of environmental and behavioral events (Critchfield et al. 1998), and these methods warrant more empirical attention by behavior analysts. Assessing Antecedents with Wearable Sensors Endogenous Antecedents Several systems use a combination of wearable passive sensors and active input by the end user to detect physiological antecedents to unhealthy behavior (Ali et al. 2012;

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

25

Crowley-Koch and Van Houten 2013; Ertin et al. 2011; Kaplan and Stone 2013; Kumar et al. 2013a; Plarre et al. 2011). For example, Boyer et al. (2012) developed the iHeal system to detect antecedents to drug use. The system consists of sensors to measure electrodermal responses, body motion, skin temperature, and (optionally) heart rate. The sensors wirelessly transmit the data to a mobile phone, and if elevated levels of activity are detected, the iHeal software triggers an EMA assessment of mood, behavior, and environmental context. The sensors and EMA data are used to iteratively train a machine learning algorithm to recognize the sensor-based profiles that may precede drug use (as opposed to other behavior like exercise). The system is still in the development phase, so no reliability or validity data are available. One goal of the iHeal system is to detect risk, as measured by the sensors, early enough to allow treatment delivery via a mobile phone before drug use occurs. Other approaches use additional sensors such as electrocardiogram (ECG), respiratoryinductive plethysmograph bands to measure relative thorax volume and respiratory frequency, global positioning systems (GPS), and accelerometers to measure physical activity (Ertin et al. 2011; Kumar et al. 2013a; Plarre et al. 2011). Many of these approaches also use machine learning algorithms, in combination with user input to report emotional states and environmental variables, to detect a profile that indicates a particular physiological state or behavior. For example, Sun et al. (2012) used galvanic skin response (GSR), ECG, accelerometers, and various machine learning algorithms to differentiate experimentally induced stress from other physical activities such as walking. They found that one algorithm was able to isolate stress from physical activity with 90 % accuracy. One key in this work was the use of accelerometers, which allowed the algorithm to learn that activityinduced physiological responses did not reflect stress responses. The approach of Sun et al. (2012) and others like it are promising, but have yet to be thoroughly tested in naturalistic contexts. To the extent that endogenous events are a function of MOs, it might be useful for future work to explicitly assess the potential MOs in addition to the sequence involving endogenous events and unhealthy behavior. Moreover, it might be useful to collect information about the full context in which endogenous responding is occurring, which means information about the components of the functional unit of analysis: MOs, discriminative stimuli, operant behavior, and consequences. Just as with overt behavior, endogenous events cannot be understood apart from the functional unit in which they participate (see Fig. 1; Thompson 2007). Although a virtue of mobile technology is that an intervention could be delivered in real-time in a naturalistic setting if some sequence were detected, the type of intervention may depend critically on the context in which the sequence occurs (Heron and Smyth 2010; Kaplan and Stone 2013). A conceptually systematic view of the interactions between endogenous events and their context will help broaden the range of possible interventions. For example, knowledge about the originating conditions of endogenous events and the consequences of unhealthy behavior may suggest interventions that remediate these conditions and consequences (Skinner 1953, p. 167). Such knowledge may even contraindicate certain interventions. If unhealthy behavior is maintained by social attention, for instance, then it may be counterproductive to provide access to peer-based support based on physiological precursors to unhealthy behavior.

26

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

Exteroceptive Antecedents Functionality present in most mobile phones can be used to assess environmental antecedents to unhealthy behavior (e.g., smoking, consuming excessive amount of alcohol or food). Mobile phones can track a user’s location via GPS (Kerr et al. 2011), which can generate automated signals when a user is entering a risky area (e.g., a bar). Microphones in mobile phones have also been used to infer whether an individual is alone or surrounded by others (Lane et al. 2011). McClernon and Roy Choudhury (2013) discuss how mobile phones could be used to detect a number of antecedents to cigarette smoking, including driving in a car (assessed via accelerometer input), the presence of other people (assessed via microphone or Bluetooth detection of other mobile devices in the area), or standing outside (assessed via camera). The authors also noted that multiple inputs could be integrated to identify an antecedent. For example, arriving at a bar, a potential antecedent to smoking, could be assessed based on a mobile phone’s clock (e.g., 10:00 p.m.), GPS, and/or accelerometer. Although these examples pertain to smoking, they also apply to detecting antecedents of other health-related operant behavior such as overeating, inactivity, or use of illicit drugs. A few researchers have harnessed technology to detect exteroceptive antecedents in the ways outlined above. Burns et al. (2011) developed a mobile phone- and Internetbased application, called Mobilyze!, to treat depression using behavioral activation therapy. Mobilyze! uses so-called context sensing techniques to predict patients’ mood, emotional, and motivational states, as well as contextual variables (e.g., activities, location, social context). Antecedent variables were derived from 38 sensors, most of which were housed in the mobile phone (e.g., GPS, accelerometer, missed call count, ambient light). In combination with EMA to identify mood, a machine learning algorithm was employed to generate individualized prediction models to identify specific user states (e.g., mood) from sensor values. In addition, based on the sensor data, Mobilyze! provided individualized behavioral activation treatment recommendations via phone and a website. Preliminary data suggest that this mHealth treatment is feasible and efficacious in treating depression (Burns et al. 2011). Another example is a mobile phone-based intervention to support recovering alcoholics (Gustafson et al. 2014). The intervention included a wide range of support services, including GPS detection of geographical areas that were identified as high risk for drinking. Upon detection of these areas via the mobile device, a message was sent to the individual inquiring whether he/she wished to be in that area. Although individuals in the mobile intervention showed lower drinking levels than a control group, it is not possible to determine whether the detection of risky areas contributed to these group differences. Assessing Health Behavior Active Input by an End User EMA requiring active input by an end user into a mobile device represents the most common method to assess health behavior (Kaplan and Stone 2013; Shiffman et al. 2008). Just as with EMA assessment of antecedents, however, the validity of EMA in measuring health behavior requires further research. Space limitations preclude a

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

27

thorough discussion of these issues in light of the heterogeneity of EMA methods, the health behavior targeted, and the standard used to evaluate EMA measures of behavior (see Shiffman et al. 2008). The lack of a true gold standard like direct observation is perhaps the greatest limitation in studies on the validity of EMA (Shiffman et al. 2008). Direct observation, however, is complicated because some unhealthy activities occur surreptitiously and at low rates (e.g., binge eating, drug use). In some cases, notably drug use, EMA can be compared to biochemical products of behavior as the standard for comparison. For example, Shiffman (2009b) found a statistically significant relation between EMA-reported cigarettes per day and breath carbon monoxide (CO; Pearson’s r=0.35). When analysis was restricted to those subjects who showed high variability in breath CO, which presumably meant greater variation in smoking behavior, the correlation improved to r=0.69. However, this study and others suggest considerable individual differences in agreement between self-reported EMA measures of drug use and biomarker levels. As just one example, Perrine et al. (1995) reported that withinsubject correlations between self-reported drinks per day and blood alcohol concentration (BAC) ranged from −0.07 to 0.92. The sources of variability may stem from either the self-report or the BAC measures, as both entail measurement error. As new, potentially more accurate, biomarkers for alcohol and other substances are developed (see below), it will be useful to use them as standards for comparison for EMA methods. In addition, for more easily observed health behavior (e.g., activity, inactivity, eating), it will be useful to further evaluate the relation between EMA and direct observation measures under controlled conditions (Thompson and Borrero 2011). Passive Input via Sensors In addition to methods that require active input by the end user, researchers and software developers are generating a plethora of new devices to assess health behavior using passive sensors (Ali et al. 2012; Crowley-Koch and Van Houten 2013; Kumar et al. 2013a; Makeyev et al. 2012; for discussion of technology to detect health status and diagnostics, see Kumar et al. 2013a and Topol 2012). The sensors are passive in that they do not require active input by an end user, but they still require the end user to interact with or wear the device. Several technologies assess discrete instances of behavior such as medication adherence (Bosworth 2012; Granger and Bosworth 2011). Medication event monitoring systems (MEMS) are pill bottles or containers that are fitted with microcircuitry. The microcircuitry records the time and date of each opening and closing of the container. This information is transmitted to research or medical personnel to track medication adherence, and it can be used to provide prompts, and monetary or social consequences to promote adherence (see below). MEMS have been used to assess adherence to a wide variety of medications such as highly active antiretroviral therapy (HAART; Krummenacher et al. 2011), analgesics (Oldenmenger et al. 2007), antipsychotics (Acosta et al. 2009; Remington et al. 2013), antidepressants (Lee et al. 2010a, b), antiepileptic medication (Brown et al. 2009), blood pressure-lowering medications (Zeller et al. 2007), and psychostimulants in children with ADHD (Charach et al. 2008). Smart blister packs accomplish the same functionality as MEMS. A blister pack houses each pill in one of many pockets in a thin card made of aluminum and paper. Instead of detecting pill container openings, they detect packet openings by using

28

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

sensors attached to the packaging, or by placing sensor “stickers” on the back of an existing blister pack (van Onzenoort et al. 2012). A limitation of MEMS and blister packs is that pill ingestion per se cannot be verified. One way to verify pill ingestion is to watch the process via video-enabled phones. The in-person equivalent to this process is called directly observed therapy (DOT), which is the model recommended by the World Health Organization to promote adherence to medication regimens such as tuberculosis treatment. DeMaio et al. (2001) used videophones to observe patients swallowing medication for tuberculosis. Adherence using video-based DOT (95 %) was equivalent to adherence using traditional DOT (97.5 %). Further, video-based DOT required only 20 h of staff time, compared to the 288 h (and 8,830 miles of travel) that would have been required had traditional DOT been used (see also Hoffman et al. 2010; Wade et al. 2012). In addition, new technologies are being explored to verify pill ingestion. For example, digital pills contain small microchips that interact with gastric juices to produce a voltage. The voltage can be detected by a small sensor adhered to the skin, similar to a Band-Aid, and the sensor can also record information about heart rate and body temperature (Zullig et al. 2014). Future research should compare MEMS with other measures to evaluate whether MEMS openings reflect pill ingestion and the conditions under which discrepancies arise. Denhaerynck et al. (2008) examined the correspondence between the patient opening an electronic bottle cap and the immediate ingestion of the prescribed dose as measured by self-report. Participants (250 adult renal transplant patients) were asked to record discrepancies between MEMS openings and the ingestion of immunosuppressive drugs throughout a 3-month period. Twenty-three patients did not use the MEMS device as prescribed and were removed from analysis. Although results revealed that participants dosed correctly on 96.3 % of days, 155 (62 %) reported discrepancies between openings and actual medication ingestion (resulting in 1,084 deviations out of 44,761 events) Twenty-eight percent of the deviations involved early decants of pills that were ingested later. Other frequently noted reasons patients gave for the discrepancies were going out, being on a trip, and having a meeting. It is possible that the large size of the MEMS device limited its portability, which may become less of a concern as devices become smaller (e.g., smart blister packs). Overall, despite some potential discrepancies between MEMS openings and actual ingestion, MEMS are still considered a “gold standard” for medication assessment (Barnett et al. 2011; Riekert and Rand 2002; Williams et al. 2013). Physical activity is another health behavior that can be monitored remotely with sensors. For example, accelerometers collect information about physical activity and inactivity by measuring changes in velocity over time (acceleration; Intille et al. 2011; King et al. 2013). This permits quantification of the intensity of physical activity. Most accelerometers estimate caloric expenditure, duration and intensity of physical activity, as well as sleep/wake patterns. The reliability and validity of these devices in estimating energy expenditure depends on several factors, such as the type of accelerometer, the location of the accelerometer on the body, the amount of time per day the device is worn, the type of activity (ambulatory versus isometric exercise), and the particular equation used to convert acceleration counts to energy expenditure (Fridlund Dunton et al. 2012; Rothney et al. 2008; see Plasqui et al. 2013 for a review).

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

29

Several accelerometers (e.g., the Actigraph triaxial accelerometer) can measure physical activity associated with most ambulatory movements (e.g., walking, running) and the absence of such activity (i.e., sedentary behavior). Thus far, most accelerometers are less capable of detecting energy expenditure associated with weight lifting, biking, and other nonambulatory activities. Although new devices are fusing sensors to measure physical activity by measuring perspiration, skin temperature, and heart rate (e.g., the BasisBand, BASIS Science, Inc.), evidence suggests that they do not improve estimation of energy expenditure (when compared to doubly labeled water estimates; Plasqui et al. 2013). One promising development is the use of new activity sensors, such as piezo-resistive sensors, which may permit detection of different activity types, such as lying, sitting or standing, active standing, walking, running, and cycling (Plasqui et al. 2013). Transient Products of Health Behavior Biomarkers can detect an increasing range of health behavior, and the technologies to detect biomarkers are becoming more affordable and practical. For example, paper microfluidics has been used to detect biomarkers of medication adherence (Lee et al. 2010b; for a review of applications of microfluidics, see Li et al. 2012). The technology involves a small test strip embedded with chemicals that react with the urine of individuals that adhere to a medication regimen (e.g., the first-line tuberculosis drug isoniazid). In one innovative application of paper microfluidics to isoniazid adherence (Barclay 2009), the test strip revealed a unique code which participants then transmitted via text to a server to verify adherence. Another example of verifying medication adherence is through breath output. Morey et al. (2013) combined a food additive (2-butanol) with a medication. After ingestion, the additive was converted to 2-butanone, which was detected in breath output. The authors found that the method was safe, feasible, and not impacted by food intake, but future work is necessary to identify whether genetic differences may impact accuracy (due to genetic polymorphisms in enzymes responsible for the conversion to 2-butaone). Technology-based methods to detect licit and illicit drug use are also becoming more practical and versatile (Bough et al. 2013). For example, Meredith et al. (2014) recently developed a prototype of a mobile phone-based breath CO monitor to detect smoking status. New alcohol breathalyzers include cameras, GPS, and face recognition software to remotely monitor intake and to authenticate the end user’s identity (Alcohol Monitoring 2013; see also Leffingwell et al. 2013). Recent drug use can also be detected in sweat (Gambelunghe et al. 2013). The Secure Continuous Remote Alcohol Monitoring (SCRAM, Alcohol Monitoring 2013) bracelet detects approximately 1 % of ingested alcohol that is excreted through the skin (Swift 2003). Although potentially more costly and invasive, new urine-based biomarkers of alcohol ingestion may increase the time window for detection relative to breath-based markers. For example, McDonell et al. (2012) used ethyl-glucuronide (EtG) in urine, which has a 2-day detection period, in a behavioral intervention to promote abstinence. Assessing Consequences with Technology EMA methods have been used to assess potential consequences of unhealthy behavior. For instance, researchers have examined whether automatic negative

30

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

reinforcement—assessed via self-report of reductions in negative affect—is associated with smoking a cigarette or binge eating (Shiffman 2009a; Haedt-Matt and Keel 2011). Research has not clearly supported a negative reinforcement process in ongoing cigarette smokers (Shiffman 2009a), nor has it revealed a reduction in negative affect following binge eating (Haedt-Matt and Keel 2011). To our knowledge, only a handful of studies have used EMA to examine the consequences after unhealthy behavior (e.g., Mitchell et al. 2014; Piasecki et al. 2008, 2011; Shiffman and Kirchner 2009). Piasecki et al. (2011) used EMA to assess the effects of alcohol consumption on subsequent cigarette smoking and the effects of cigarette smoking on subsequent alcohol consumption. Current smokers who reported frequently drinking alcohol (N=259) monitored their daily experiences for 21 days. The authors found that alcohol enhanced the pleasurable consequences of smoking more strongly than smoking enhanced the pleasurable consequences of alcohol, but both substances produced small increases in selfreported pleasure derived from the other substance. Given that the effects were small and only group averaged data were evaluated, it would be interesting to examine individual differences in the effects of these substances. Conceptually, even though the EMA method was designed to assess consequences of consumption, these value enhancing effects might be interpreted in light of the MO functions of these substances (Raiff and Dallery 2008). Assessing the roles of consequences will enrich our understanding of the determinants of health behavior and could lead to more tailored, function-based interventions (Axelrod 1991). Indeed, an operant analysis involves idiographic assessment of the entire functional unit of analysis. Thus far, no technology-based study of health behavior has included idiographic assessment of consequences. Encouragingly, however, and perhaps by virtue of advances in technology, several health behavior researchers have advocated for within-subject analysis of the antecedent and consequent variables governing healthy and unhealthy behavior (Kanning et al. 2013; Riley et al. 2011; Shiffman 2014). Behavior analysts have expertise in using methods to not only detect instances of antecedents, behavior, and consequences, but also to assess whether these instances are functionally related to one another and not merely correlated (e.g., Beavers et al. 2013; Hanley et al. 2003; McComas et al. 2009). For example, contingency space analysis, which is a quantitative approach to identifying contingent relations, may help identify variables maintaining unhealthy behavior in naturalistic environments (see Martens et al. 2014). Moreover, behavior analysts have expertise in linking idiographic assessment to treatment, which is also a growing interest among health behavior researchers (e.g., Heron and Smyth 2010; Kaplan and Stone 2013; Riley et al. 2011). Thus, the extent to which functional assessment could be used to develop more targeted, personalized technology-based treatments is a compelling empirical question for behavior analysts interested in health behavior.

Technology-Based Interventions to Promote Health Internet and Mobile Interventions There is growing empirical support for Internet and mobile interventions for smoking (Dallery et al. 2014a), weight loss (Valle and Tate, in press), and physical activity

BEHAV ANALYST (2015) 38:19–49

31

(Davies et al. 2012). These interventions are usually derived from behavioral or cognitive-behavioral therapies and typically are interactive and include feedback, goal setting, and frequent monitoring of progress during treatment. Most systematic studies and meta-analyses have found that these interventions are more efficacious than standard care (nontechnology-based) control conditions. In an effort to better understand the behavior change components involved in technology-based interventions, Valle and Tate (in press) reviewed 21 studies targeting dietary intake, weight loss, and physical activity. The authors focused on studies that compared technological delivery of treatment components to a technology-based control condition (e.g., a website with and without access to a component such as social support). They found that 19 studies used self-monitoring, 16 used feedback and tailoring (e.g., individualized goal setting based on changes in behavior relative to baseline levels), 14 used social support and social networking, and 19 used reminders or prompts to engage with intervention content. Relatively few interventions (

A New Frontier: Integrating Behavioral and Digital Technology to Promote Health Behavior.

Modifiable behavioral risk factors such as cigarette smoking, physical inactivity, and obesity contribute to over 40 % of premature deaths in the USA...
568KB Sizes 0 Downloads 11 Views